Reversal of innate aversions: attempts to induce a preference for chili peppers in rats.

Although humans frequently develop preferences for innately unpalatable bitter or irritant substances, such preferences are extremely rare in animals. An attempt was made to understand the nature of this difference by systematic experiments with laboratory rats, with chili pepper as the unpalatable substance. In parallel with major aspects of the human experience with chili pepper, rats were exposed to it as a flavoring in all their food for periods up to 11 mo from birth, without significant preference enhancement. Gradual introduction of chili into the diet also had no effect, nor did a series of poisoning and safety experiences designed to teach the rats that only chili-flavored foods were safe to eat. A sequence of seven pairings of chili-flavored diet with prompt recovery from thiamine deficiency did significantly attenuate the innate aversion and may have induced a chili preference in at least one case. Extensive experience with chili did not reliably make rats much less sensitive to its oral effects. The only reliable way to eliminate chili aversion in rats is to destroy their chemical irritant sense, which was accomplished in one group of rats. It is concluded that in contrast to humans, it is extremely difficult to reverse innate aversions in rats.

[1]  Paul Rozin,et al.  The nature and acquisition of a preference for chili pepper by humans , 1980 .

[2]  C. Gross,et al.  Color categories in macaques. , 1979, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  N. Walton,et al.  A rat alcoholism model in a free choice situation. , 1977, Behavioral biology.

[4]  J. Maga,et al.  The role of sulfur compounds in food flavor part I: Thiazoles , 1975 .

[5]  S. Maier,et al.  Preferences for tastes paired with recovery from thiamine deficiency in rats: appetitive conditioning or learned safety? , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  P. Capretta,et al.  Establishment of a flavor preference in rats: importance of nursing and weaning experience. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[7]  L. Parker,et al.  Conditioned preferences in the rat with an unnatural need state: morphine withdrawal. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[8]  P. Rozin,et al.  Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. , 1971, Psychological review.

[9]  R. Kovner,et al.  Cue properties vs palatability of flavors in avoidance learning , 1970 .

[10]  P. Rozin Specific aversions as a component of specific hungers. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[11]  R. A. Koelling,et al.  Conditioning with Delayed Vitamin Injections , 1967, Science.

[12]  E. R. Siqueland EXPERIMENTAL MODIFICATION OF TASTE PREFERENCE. , 1965, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[13]  A. Cowey,et al.  The aetiology of food reward in monkeys , 1963 .

[14]  C. Pfaffmann,et al.  Early experience and taste aversion. , 1959, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[15]  P. Young Appetite, palatability and feeding habit; a critical review. , 1948, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  T. Inman,et al.  Alcohol as Food , 1861 .

[17]  P. Rozin The Selection of Foods by Rats, Humans, and Other Animals , 1976 .

[18]  W. Hankins,et al.  The Evolution of Bitter and the Acquisition of Toxiphobia , 1975 .

[19]  L. M. Beidler Phylogenetic Emergence of Sour Taste , 1975 .

[20]  N. Mello,et al.  A review of methods to induce alcohol addiction in animals. , 1973, Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior.

[21]  D. Hilker,et al.  Free choice consumption of spiced diets by rats. , 1967, The Journal of nutrition.

[22]  J. Lefèvre ALCOHOL AS FOOD. , 1899, Science.