Learning a New Bimanual Coordination Pattern: Interlimb Interactions, Attentional Focus, and Transfer

ABSTRACT Because bimanual coordinative stability is governed by interlimb coupling, we examined how learning a new pattern (90°) was reflected in changes in the underlying interlimb interactions. Three interlimb interaction sources were distinguished: integrated timing of feedforward control signals, error corrections based on perceived relative phase, and phase entrainment by contralateral afference. By comparing 4 tasks that involve these interactions to a different extent, changes in the stabilizing contributions of these coupling sources could be studied. Furthermore, we studied how the learning process and changes in the underlying interactions were influenced by attentional focus (internal vs. external), and we examined retention of the learned pattern and transfer to the mirror-symmetrical pattern (270°). Results showed that stability and accuracy of the new pattern increased significantly with learning, due to improved integrated timing and error correction. Integrated timing improved first, possibly providing a reference frame for the error corrections that subsequently became more effective. Despite some qualitative differences in the learning process, neither performance of the learned pattern nor the underlying interlimb interactions was influenced by attentional focus. Whereas the learned pattern improved directly after practice, transfer followed later, suggesting that a more general representation was formed at a slower rate after practice.

[1]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands , 1980, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  J. Kelso,et al.  Phase Transitions and Critical Fluctuations in Rhythmic Coordination of Ipsilateral Hand and Foot. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[3]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[4]  J. Born,et al.  Sleep forms memory for finger skills , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Internal and External Focus of Attention in a Novice Form Sport , 2011, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[6]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Bimanual 1:1 with 90° continuous relative phase: difficult or easy! , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  J. Kelso,et al.  Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Y. Laufer,et al.  Effect of focus of attention on transfer of a postural control task following an ankle sprain. , 2007, The Journal of orthopaedic and sports physical therapy.

[9]  Stefan Panzer,et al.  Inter-manual transfer and practice: coding of simple motor sequences. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[10]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination in patients with Parkinson’s disease: motor learning deficits and the importance of augmented information feedback , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Melvyn Roerdink,et al.  Visual and musculoskeletal underpinnings of anchoring in rhythmic visuo-motor tracking , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Error correction in bimanual coordination benefits from bilateral muscle activity: evidence from kinesthetic tracking , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  G. Wulf,et al.  An external focus of attention enhances balance learning in older adults. , 2010, Gait & posture.

[15]  W. Prinz,et al.  Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. , 1990 .

[17]  P J Beek,et al.  Movement-Related Sensory Feedback Mediates the Learning of a New Bimanual Relative Phase Pattern , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[19]  R. Stickgold,et al.  It's practice, with sleep, that makes perfect: implications of sleep-dependent learning and plasticity for skill performance. , 2005, Clinics in sports medicine.

[20]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Effects of focus of attention depend on golfers' skill , 2003, Journal of sports sciences.

[21]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[22]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Production of polyrhythms. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Impossible is nothing: 5:3 and 4:3 multi-frequency bimanual coordination , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Development of temporal and spatial bimanual coordination during childhood. , 2012, Motor control.

[26]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Motor learning with augmented feedback: modality-dependent behavioral and neural consequences. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Unraveling interlimb interactions underlying bimanual coordination. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  The learning of 90° continuous relative phase with and without Lissajous feedback: external and internally generated bimanual coordination. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[29]  Chaoyi Wang,et al.  Overcoming the guidance effect in motor skill learning: feedback all the time can be beneficial , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  C. Shea,et al.  Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning , 2003, Psychological research.

[32]  Winona Snapp-Childs,et al.  Perceptual learning immediately yields new stable motor coordination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[34]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Bilateral phase entrainment by movement-elicited afference contributes equally to the stability of in-phase and antiphase coordination , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Representation of movement sequences is related to task characteristics. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[36]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping: Asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. , 1995 .

[37]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  G. Wulf,et al.  An External Focus of Attention Enhances Golf Shot Accuracy in Beginners and Experts , 2007, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[39]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Acquiring a Novel Coordination Skill without Practicing the Correct Motor Commands , 2010, Journal of motor behavior.

[40]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Active versus Passive Training of a Complex Bimanual Task: Is Prescriptive Proprioceptive Information Sufficient for Inducing Motor Learning? , 2012, PloS one.

[41]  G. Wulf,et al.  The learning advantages of an external focus of attention in golf. , 1999, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[42]  G. Deuschl,et al.  Sleep consolidates the effector-independent representation of a motor skill , 2010, Neuroscience.

[43]  Arthur S Reber,et al.  The locus of focus: The effect of switching from a preferred to a non-preferred focus of attention , 2008, Journal of sports sciences.

[44]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[45]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Contextual interference: single task versus multi-task learning. , 2004, Motor control.

[46]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Frequency-induced changes in interlimb interactions: Increasing manifestations of closed-loop control , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[47]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation , 2003, Nature.

[48]  R. Gray Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking, and slumps. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[49]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  John J. Buchanan,et al.  Learning a single limb multijoint coordination pattern: the impact of a mechanical constraint on the coordination dynamics of learning and transfer , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  P G Zanone,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  Dana Maslovat,et al.  Feedback Effects on Learning a Novel Bimanual Coordination Pattern: Support for the Guidance Hypothesis , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[53]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[54]  R. Carson,et al.  The acquisition of movement skills: practice enhances the dynamic stability of bimanual coordination. , 2001, Human movement science.

[55]  J. Porter,et al.  Increasing the Distance of an External Focus of Attention Enhances Standing Long Jump Performance , 2012, Journal of strength and conditioning research.

[56]  R. Gray,et al.  Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[57]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[58]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Attentional loads associated with interlimb interactions underlying rhythmic bimanual coordination , 2008, Cognition.

[59]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .