Serial position effects and lexical activation in spelling: Evidence from a single case study

Abstract A single case study of a ‘deep dysgraphic’ patient is reported. The majority of her errors consist of non-word and fragment responses in which initial letters tend to be correctly produced (e.g. book b, dentist dentant). The probability of making an error at a given position increases linearly from word beginning to word end for all spelling tasks requiring lexical access. This serial position effect cannot be attributed to damage to the graphemic buffer, since a different pattern is found in a buffer-taxing task which does not require lexical access. It is also unlikely to reflect a neglect deficit, since the serial position effect is related to ordinal positions rather than positions left or right of word-centre. It is argued that the patient's pattern reflects incomplete activation of lexical-orthographic representations. Our data are consistent with models of lexical activation in which the activation of letter nodes is a function of ordinal position.

[1]  A. W. Ellis Normality and pathology in cognitive functions , 1982 .

[2]  E. Warrington,et al.  CATEGORY SPECIFIC ACCESS DYSPHASIA , 1983 .

[3]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibitory Mechanisms of Neural and Cognitive Control: Applications to Selective Attention and Sequential Action , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Selective impairment of semantics in lexical processing , 1990 .

[5]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[6]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Levels of representation, co-ordinate frames, and unilateral neglect , 1990 .

[7]  David Howard,et al.  Why don't Broca's aphasics cue themselves? an investigation of phonemic cueing and tip of the tongue information , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[9]  N. Burgess,et al.  Toward a network model of the articulatory loop , 1992, Connectionist psychology: A text with readings.

[10]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Refractory semantics in global aphasia: on semantic organisation and the access-storage distinction in neuropsychology. , 1995, Memory.

[11]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .

[12]  J. Marshall,et al.  Line bisection in a case of visual neglect: Psychophysical studies with implications for theory , 1990 .

[13]  Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al.  A variety of reading disability associated with right hemisphere lesions , 1962 .

[14]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Spatially Specific Deficits in Processing Graphemic Representations in Reading and Writing , 1995, Brain and Language.

[15]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[16]  A. Jensen,et al.  Spelling errors and the serial-position effect. , 1962 .

[17]  A. Mazzucchi,et al.  Selective impairment of the Graphemic Buffer in acquired dysgraphia: A case study , 1988, Brain and Language.

[18]  A. Kertesz The Western Aphasia Battery , 1982 .

[19]  O. L. Zangwill,et al.  A STUDY OF DYSLEXIA , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[21]  James E. Mendenhall An analysis of spelling errors : a study of factors associated with word difficulty , 1930 .

[22]  T Shallice,et al.  Semantic access dyslexia. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  David Glasspool,et al.  Can neuropsychological evidence inform connectionist modelling? Analyses of spelling , 1995 .

[24]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .

[25]  Robert B. Katz,et al.  Positional dyslexia , 1989, Brain and Language.

[26]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The role of the Graphemic Buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia , 1987, Cognition.

[27]  D. Denny-Brown,et al.  The significance of perceptual rivalry resulting from parietal lesion. , 1952, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .

[29]  Robert B. Katz,et al.  Limited Retention of Information in the Graphemic Buffer , 1991, Cortex.

[30]  Mp. Departz Deficit of the graphemic buffer: Effects of a written lexical segmentation strategy , 1995 .

[31]  Peter F. MacNeilage,et al.  Typing Errors as Clues to Serial Ordering Mechanisms in Language Behaviour , 1964 .

[32]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Simulating a Skilled Typist: A Study of Skilled Cognitive-Motor Performance , 1982, Cogn. Sci..

[33]  George Houghton,et al.  The problem of serial order: a neural network model of sequence learning and recall , 1990 .

[34]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The status of double letters in Graphemic representations , 1996 .

[35]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .

[36]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Where Do Semantic Errors Come From? , 1990, Cortex.

[37]  R. Lesser Superior oral to written spelling : evidence for separate buffers ? , 1990 .

[38]  Chris Mellish,et al.  Current research in natural language generation , 1990 .

[39]  Roberto Cubelli,et al.  Different patterns of dissociation in unilateral spatial neglect , 1991, Brain and Cognition.

[40]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Interruption of phonological coding in conduction aphasia , 1984, Brain and Language.

[41]  Nadine Martin,et al.  Specifying the nature of the production impairment in a conductionxs aphasic: A case study , 1987 .

[42]  D. Margolin The neuropsychology of Writing and Spelling: Semantic, Phonological, Motor, and Perceptual Processes , 1984, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[43]  E K Warrington,et al.  The dissociation of visuospatial neglect and neglect dyslexia. , 1987, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[44]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Disturbances in conceptual space involving language and speech. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Wernicke's aphasia and normal language processing: A case study in cognitive neuropsychology , 1983, Cognition.

[46]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Slips of the Pen. , 1979 .

[47]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Disorders of phonological encoding , 1992, Cognition.

[48]  K Bock,et al.  The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[49]  William Badecker,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia , 1994 .

[50]  David Howard Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (The) , 1992 .

[51]  A. Young,et al.  “Neglect dyslexia” and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords , 1987 .

[52]  F. M. Hatfield Visual and phonological factors in acquired dysgraphia , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Graphemic Buffer and attentional mechanisms , 1989, Brain and Language.

[54]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Phonological mediation and the graphemic buffer disorder in spelling: cross-language differences? , 1996, Cognition.

[55]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[56]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Remediation of acquired dysgraphia as a technique for testing interpretations of deficits , 1993 .

[57]  A. Caramazza,et al.  On the distinction between deficits of access and deficits of storage : a question of theory , 1993 .

[58]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .

[59]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Lexical decision and aphasia: Evidence for semantic processing , 1981, Brain and Language.

[60]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient , 1990, Nature.

[61]  Beverly Y. Kooi,et al.  Spelling errors and the serial-position effect. , 1965, Journal of educational psychology.

[62]  L. Trojano,et al.  Pure Dysgraphia with Relative Sparing of Lower-Case Writing , 1994, Cortex.

[63]  E K Warrington,et al.  Concrete word dyslexia. , 1981, British journal of psychology.

[64]  G. Ettlinger,et al.  THE OCCASIONAL “INDEPENDENCE” OF DYSLEXIA AND DYSGRAPHIA FROM DYSPHASIA , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[65]  John J. L. Morton,et al.  Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .

[66]  Barbara A. Wilson,et al.  A rose is a rose or a nose: A deficit in initial letter identification , 1990 .

[67]  A. Ellis,et al.  A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia. Implications for psychological models of word retrieval. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[68]  R. Logie,et al.  Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words , 1980 .

[69]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[70]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations , 1990, Cognition.

[71]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[72]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Computational approaches to normal and impaired spelling , 1994 .

[73]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Age of acquisition and lexical processing in aphasia: a case study , 1994 .

[74]  J. Morton The logogen model and orthographic structure , 1980 .