Physiological evidence of interaction of first‐ and second‐order motion processes in the human visual system: A magnetoencephalographic study

Humans have several mechanisms for the visual perception of motion, including one that is luminance‐based (first‐order) and another that is luminance‐independent (second‐order). Recent psychophysical studies have suggested that significant interaction occurs between these two neural processes. We investigated whether such interactions are represented as neural activity measured by magnetoencephalography (MEG). The second‐order motion of a drifting sinusoidal grating, which is defined by the speed of the dot motion, did not generate a response. Apparent motion (AM) of the square area, defined by the speed of randomly moving dots, evoked a magnetic response whose latency and amplitude changed with the distance that the area moved (a second‐order characteristic), though the response properties were significantly different from those for the first‐order AM. AM, defined by both first‐ and second‐order attributes, evoked an MEG response and the latencies and the amplitudes were distributed between those for the first‐ and second‐order motions. The cortical source of the response was estimated to be around MT+. The results show a distinct difference in the neural processing of the second‐order motion that cannot be explained by the difference in visibility, and they indicate that the interaction of the neural processes underlying first‐ and second‐order motion detection occurs before the MEG response. Our study provides the first physiological evidence of a neural interaction between the two types of early motion detection. Hum. Brain Mapp. 20:158–167, 2003. © 2003 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  H Suzuki,et al.  Human cortical area responding to stimuli in apparent motion , 1997, Neuroreport.

[2]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[3]  Ryusuke Kakigi,et al.  Human cortical response to incoherent motion on a background of coherent motion , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[4]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Sensitivity to modulations of luminance and contrast in visual white noise: separate mechanisms with similar behaviour , 1999, Vision Research.

[5]  J. Sarvas Basic mathematical and electromagnetic concepts of the biomagnetic inverse problem. , 1987, Physics in medicine and biology.

[6]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order motion in human vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[7]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Does early non-linearity account for second-order motion? , 1999, Vision Research.

[8]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Impaired visual motion perception in the contralateral hemifield following unilateral posterior cerebral lesions in humans. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  U. Ilg,et al.  Processing of second-order motion stimuli in primate middle temporal area and medial superior temporal area. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[10]  P. McOwan,et al.  A computational model of the analysis of some first-order and second-order motion patterns by simple and complex cells , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[12]  N Osaka,et al.  Inefficient visual search for second-order motion. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[13]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  S. Nishida,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.

[15]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Dual multiple-scale processing for motion in the human visual System , 1997, Vision Research.

[16]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A new anatomical landmark for reliable identification of human area V5/MT: a quantitative analysis of sulcal patterning. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Motion defined exclusively by second-order characteristics does not evoke optokinetic nystagmus , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[18]  Z L Lu,et al.  Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[19]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Interaction between primary and secondary mechanisms in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[20]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain , 1993 .

[21]  U. Ilg,et al.  Initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements to first-order and second-order motion stimuli , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  J. Victor,et al.  Evoked potential and psychophysical analysis of Fourier and non-Fourier motion mechanisms , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  Marcello G P Rosa,et al.  Physiological responses of New World monkey V1 neurons to stimuli defined by coherent motion. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  L M Vaina,et al.  A lesion of cortical area V2 selectively impairs the perception of the direction of first‐order visual motion , 2000, Neuroreport.

[25]  Fixation and Attention in Apparent Motion , 1977 .

[26]  Adriane E Seiffert,et al.  Functional MRI studies of human visual motion perception: texture, luminance, attention and after-effects. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  L. P. O'Keefe,et al.  Processing of first- and second-order motion signals by neurons in area MT of the macaque monkey , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[28]  N. Scott-Samuel,et al.  No local cancellation between directionally opposed first-order and second-order motion signals , 2000, Vision Research.

[29]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[30]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[31]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Interactions between first- and second-order motion revealed by optokinetic nystagmus , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  C. Baker Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Detection and Discrimination of First- and Second-Order Motion in Patients with Unilateral Brain Damage , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  N E Scott-Samuel,et al.  First-order and second-order signals combine to improve perceptual accuracy. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[35]  T. L. Lewis,et al.  Longer VEP latencies and slower reaction times to the onset of second-order motion than to the onset of first-order motion , 2003, Vision Research.

[36]  R. Kakigi,et al.  Timing of motion representation in the human visual system , 1998, Brain Research.

[37]  Y. Okada,et al.  Genesis of MEG signals in a mammalian CNS structure. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  A. Cowey,et al.  Perception of first‐ and second‐order motion: Separable neurological mechanisms? , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[39]  Ryusuke Kakigi,et al.  Human cortical responses to coherent and incoherent motion as measured by magnetoencephalography , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[40]  A. Cowey,et al.  Impairment of the perception of second order motion but not first order motion in a patient with unilateral focal brain damage , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[41]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[42]  R Kakigi,et al.  Human visual motion areas determined individually by magnetoencephalography and 3D magnetic resonance imaging , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[43]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Visual detection of motion speed in humans: spatiotemporal analysis by fMRI and MEG , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[44]  J. Hennig,et al.  The Processing of First- and Second-Order Motion in Human Visual Cortex Assessed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  G. Sperling,et al.  Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[46]  N R Burns,et al.  Interaction of first- and second-order direction in motion-defined motion. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[47]  C L Baker,et al.  A processing stream in mammalian visual cortex neurons for non-Fourier responses. , 1993, Science.

[48]  E. Somersalo,et al.  Visualization of Magnetoencephalographic Data Using Minimum Current Estimates , 1999, NeuroImage.