Functionally referential alarm calls in noisy miners communicate about predator behaviour
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Andrea M.-K. Bierema,et al. On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling , 2013 .
[2] D. Dow. AGONISTIC AND SPACING BEHAVIOUR OF THE NOISY MINER MANORINA MELANOCEPHALA, A COMMUNALLY BREEDING HONEYEATER , 2008 .
[3] S. Siegel,et al. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[4] D. Dow. Displays of the honeyeater Manorina melanocephala. , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.
[5] K. Zuberbühler. Referential labelling in Diana monkeys , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[6] R. Seyfarth,et al. Vervets revisited: A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[7] D. Dow,et al. Cooperative Breeding in Birds: Noisy Miners: variations on the theme of communality , 1990 .
[8] Charlotte E. Taylor,et al. Does the presence of grevilleas and eucalypts in urban gardens influence the distribution and foraging ecology of Noisy Miners? , 2009 .
[9] G. Klump,et al. Acoustic Behaviour of Birds and Mammals in the Predator Context; I. Factors Affecting the Structure of Alarm Signals. II. The Functional Significance and Evolution of Alarm Signals , 2010 .
[10] Christopher S. Evans,et al. Individual distinctiveness in the mobbing call of a cooperative bird, the noisy miner Manorina melanocephala , 2009 .
[11] C. Mary,et al. Heterospecific information about predation risk influences exploratory behavior , 2012 .
[12] Toshitaka N Suzuki,et al. Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls , 2016, Nature Communications.
[13] A. Agresti. An introduction to categorical data analysis , 1997 .
[14] K. Sanderson,et al. Perception of terrestrial and aerial alarm calls by honeyeaters and falcons , 2000 .
[15] Robert D Magrath,et al. A micro-geography of fear: learning to eavesdrop on alarm calls of neighbouring heterospecifics , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[16] C. N. Slobodchikoff,et al. Perceptual specificity in the alarm calls of Gunnison's prairie dogs , 2006, Behavioural Processes.
[17] C. Catterall,et al. A particular case and a general pattern: hyperaggressive behaviour by one species may mediate avifaunal decreases in fragmented Australian forests , 2003 .
[18] P. J. Higgins,et al. Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic birds. Volume 5: trant-Flycatchers to chats , 2001 .
[19] M. Maron. Threshold effect of eucalypt density on an aggressive avian competitor , 2007 .
[20] L. Barrett,et al. Responses of vervet monkeys in large troops to terrestrial and aerial predator alarm calls , 2014 .
[21] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Wild Birds Learn to Eavesdrop on Heterospecific Alarm Calls , 2015, Current Biology.
[22] Michael Griesser,et al. Referential Calls Signal Predator Behavior in a Group-Living Bird Species , 2008, Current Biology.
[23] R. Montgomerie,et al. Mating system of the cooperatively breeding noisy miner Manorina melanocephala, as revealed by DNA profiling , 1995, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[24] P. Marler,et al. Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate , 1980, Animal Behaviour.
[25] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Semantic communication in birds: evidence from field research over the past two decades , 2016, Ecological Research.
[26] Christopher S. Evans,et al. Essay on Contemporary Issues in Ethology: Variation among Mammalian Alarm Call Systems and the Problem of Meaning in Animal Signals , 2010 .
[27] L. A. Grieves,et al. Joint-nesting smooth-billed anis, Crotophaga ani, use a functionally referential alarm call system , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[28] Andy P. Field,et al. Discovering Statistics Using Ibm Spss Statistics , 2017 .
[29] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[30] Andrew Meade,et al. Detecting Regular Sound Changes in Linguistics as Events of Concerted Evolution , 2015, Current Biology.
[31] K. Zuberbühler. Chapter 8 Survivor Signals , 2009 .
[32] A. Radford,et al. Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences , 2015, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[33] W. Davis,et al. HANDBOOK OF AUSTRALIAN, NEW ZEALAND, AND ANTARCTIC BIRDS, VOLUME 4: PARROTS TO DOLLARBIRD , 2001 .
[34] P. Marler,et al. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. , 1980, Science.
[35] Alan Lill,et al. How Noisy Does a Noisy Miner Have to Be? Amplitude Adjustments of Alarm Calls in an Avian Urban ‘Adapter’ , 2012, PloS one.
[36] Robert M. Seyfarth,et al. Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[37] K. Sanderson,et al. Alarm Vocalisations in Australian Birds: Convergent Characteristics and Phylogenetic Differences , 1994 .
[38] Michael L. Avery,et al. The versatility of graded acoustic measures in classification of predation threats by the tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor: exploring a mixed framework for threat communication , 2010 .
[39] Brandon C. Wheeler,et al. The blurred boundaries of functional reference: a response to Scarantino & Clay , 2015, Animal Behaviour.
[40] C. Catterall,et al. BUSHLAND MODIFICATION AND STYLES OF URBAN DEVELOPMENT : THEIR EFFECTS ON BIRDS IN SOUTH-EAST QUEENSLAND , 1998 .
[41] M. Manser,et al. The acoustic structure of suricates' alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[42] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. Conceptual Semantics in a Nonhuman Primate , 1999 .
[43] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[44] D. Dow. Indiscriminate Interspecific Aggression Leading to Almost Sole Occupancy of Space by a Single Species of Bird , 1977 .
[45] T. Caro,et al. Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals , 2006 .
[46] U. Stegmann. Animal Communication Theory: Information and Influence , 2018 .
[47] Robert D Magrath,et al. Recognition of other species' aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[48] Kurt Hammerschmidt,et al. Functionally Referential Alarm Calls in Tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) – Evidence from Playback Experiments , 2006 .
[49] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. The central importance of information in studies of animal communication , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[50] M. Manser,et al. The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[51] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Communicating about danger: urgency alarm calling in a bird , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[52] B. Tabachnick,et al. Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .
[53] Simon W. Townsend,et al. Functionally Referential Communication in Mammals: The Past, Present and the Future , 2013 .
[54] M. Considine,et al. Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic birds. Volume 2: raptors to lapwings , 2007 .
[55] M. Ryan,et al. What do animal signals mean? , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[56] P. Kappeler,et al. Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system , 2002, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[57] Benjamin J. Pitcher,et al. A mutual understanding? Interspecific responses by birds to each other's aerial alarm calls , 2007 .