Variations in dietary iron alter behavior in developing rats.

Iron deficiency in children is associated with retardation in growth and cognitive development, and the effects on cognition may be irreversible, even with treatment. Excessive iron has also been associated with neurological disease, especially in reference to the increased iron content in the brains of Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease patients. This study evaluated the effects of dietary iron deficiency and excess iron on physical activity in rats. The animal model used is developmentally sensitive and permits control of the timing as well as the duration of the nutritional insult. Hence, to study the effects of early, late and long-term iron deficiency or excess iron (supplementation), rats were either made iron deficient or supplemented on postnatal day (PND) 10-21, PND 21-35 and PND 10-35. Some iron-deficient rats were iron repleted between PND 21-35. Different measures of motor activity were taken at PND 14, 17, 20, 27 and 34. Iron-deficient and iron-supplemented rats showed decreased activity and stereotypic behavior; this was apparent for any onset and duration of the nutritional insult. Recovery from iron deficiency did not normalize these functional variables, showing that the deleterious effects of early iron deficiency persist despite subsequent adequate treatment. This study demonstrates that iron deficiency in early life leads to irreversible behavioral changes. The biological bases for these behavioral alterations are not readily apparent, because iron therapy rapidly reverses the iron losses in all brain regions.

[1]  B. Lozoff,et al.  Iron and learning potential in childhood. , 1989, Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine.

[2]  I. Izquierdo Different forms of post-training memory processing. , 1989, Behavioral and neural biology.

[3]  M. Bloomfield,et al.  The effects of iron deficiency on brain biogenic monoamine biochemistry and function in rats , 1980, Neuropharmacology.

[4]  E. Larkin,et al.  Importance of Fetal and Neonatal Iron: Adequacy for Normal Development of Central Nervous System , 1990 .

[5]  G. Brittenham,et al.  Behavioral aspects of iron deficiency. , 1986, Progress in hematology.

[6]  P. J. Howanitz,et al.  Effect of iron therapy on behavior performance in nonanemic, iron-deficient infants. , 1983, Pediatrics.

[7]  F. Oski,et al.  The effects of therapy on the developmental scores of iron-deficient infants. , 1978 .

[8]  J. Beard,et al.  Abnormal rat brain monoamine metabolism in iron deficiency anemia , 1995 .

[9]  J. Beard,et al.  In vivo dopamine metabolism is altered in iron-deficient anemic rats. , 1997, The Journal of nutrition.

[10]  J. Weinberg,et al.  Iron deficiency during early development in the rat: Behavioral and physiological consequences , 1980, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[11]  D. Ben-shachar,et al.  Nutritional iron and dopamine binding sites in the rat brain , 1982, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[12]  L. Johnson,et al.  Severe or marginal iron deficiency affects spontaneous physical activity in rats. , 1994, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[13]  E. Pollitt,et al.  Reversal of developmental delays in iron-deficient anaemic infants treated with iron , 1993, The Lancet.

[14]  M. Garrido,et al.  Evidence of altered central nervous system development in infants with iron deficiency anemia at 6 mo: delayed maturation of auditory brainstem responses. , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[15]  M. Borel,et al.  The impact of varying degrees of iron nutriture on several functional consequences of iron deficiency in rats. , 1991, The Journal of nutrition.

[16]  E. Pollitt Iron deficiency and cognitive function. , 1993, Annual review of nutrition.

[17]  E. Morgan,et al.  Transferrin and Iron Uptake by the Brain: Effects of Altered Iron Status , 1991, Journal of neurochemistry.

[18]  D. Ben-shachar,et al.  Effect of Iron Chelators on Dopamine D2 Receptors , 1985, Journal of neurochemistry.

[19]  P. G. Reeves,et al.  AIN-93 purified diets for laboratory rodents: final report of the American Institute of Nutrition ad hoc writing committee on the reformulation of the AIN-76A rodent diet. , 1993, The Journal of nutrition.

[20]  B. Costall,et al.  Differentiation of the dopamine mechanisms mediating stereotyped behaviour and hyperactivity in the nucleus accumbens and caudate‐putamen , 1977, The Journal of pharmacy and pharmacology.

[21]  J. Beard,et al.  Iron deficiency alters dopamine transporter functioning in rat striatum. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.

[22]  T. Walter Impact of iron deficiency on cognition in infancy and childhood. , 1993, European journal of clinical nutrition.

[23]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  GABAA receptor modulation of memory: the role of endogenous benzodiazepines. , 1991, Trends in pharmacological sciences.

[24]  P. D. James,et al.  John Treherne (1929–1989) , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  J. Connor,et al.  Altered monamine metabolism in caudate-putamen of iron-deficient rats , 1994, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[26]  M. Youdim,et al.  Biogenic monoamine metabolism and functional activity in iron-deficient rats: behavioural correlates. , 2008, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[27]  J. Beard Feed Efficiency and Norepinephrine Turnover in Iron Deficiency , 1987, Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine. Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine.

[28]  J. Brandt,et al.  Randomised study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in non-anaemic iron-deficient adolescent girls , 1996, The Lancet.

[29]  J. Connor,et al.  Variations in dietary iron alter brain iron metabolism in developing rats. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.

[30]  J. Connor,et al.  Regional brain iron, ferritin and transferrin concentrations during iron deficiency and iron repletion in developing rats. , 1997, The Journal of nutrition.

[31]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Neonatal iron exposure induces neurobehavioural dysfunctions in adult mice. , 1999, Toxicology and applied pharmacology.

[32]  S. Iversen,et al.  Behavioral implications of dopaminergic neurons in the mesolimbic system. , 1977, Advances in biochemical psychopharmacology.

[33]  M. Golub,et al.  Chronic marginal iron intakes during early development in mice alter brain iron concentrations and behavior despite postnatal iron supplementation. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.

[34]  H. Anisman,et al.  Alteration of exploration and the response to food associated cues after treatment with pimozide , 1983, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[35]  D. Ben-shachar,et al.  Selective Alteration in Blood‐Brain Barrier and Insulin Transport in Iron‐Deficient Rats , 1988, Journal of neurochemistry.

[36]  T. Robbins,et al.  Functional studies of the central catecholamines. , 1982, International review of neurobiology.

[37]  E. Larkin,et al.  Effect of prenatal iron deficiency on myelination in rat pups. , 1986, The American journal of pathology.

[38]  B. Lozoff,et al.  Iron-deficiency anemia and infant development: effects of extended oral iron therapy. , 1996, The Journal of pediatrics.

[39]  M. Youdim,et al.  Iron deficiency-induced circadian rhythm reversal of dopaminergic-mediated behaviours and thermoregulation in rats. , 1981, European journal of pharmacology.

[40]  B. Lozoff,et al.  Brain iron and behavior of rats are not normalized by treatment of iron deficiency anemia during early development. , 1996, The Journal of nutrition.

[41]  D. McClish,et al.  Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. , 1998, Child development.