Disorder-specific neuroanatomical correlates of attentional bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, and hypochondriasis.

CONTEXT Attentional bias to disease-relevant emotional cues is considered to be pathogenetically relevant in anxiety disorders. OBJECTIVE To investigate functional neural correlates and disease specificity of attentional bias across different anxiety disorders. DESIGN A cognitive and emotional Stroop task, consisting of congruent and incongruent color words, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD)-related and panic-related negative words, and neutral words, was used in 3 patient groups and a control group during functional magnetic resonance imaging. SETTING Academic outpatient department for anxiety disorders. PATIENTS AND PARTICIPANTS Medication-free patients with OCD (n = 16), panic disorder (PD) (n = 15), and hypochondriasis (n = 13) and 19 controls. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE Voxel-wise analyses of cerebral blood flow changes for contrasts of interest (incongruent vs congruent color words, OCD-related vs neutral words, and panic-related vs neutral words) within and between groups. RESULTS During incongruent vs congruent color naming, all patient groups recruited additional posterior brain regions relative to controls, but performance was impaired only in OCD. In OCD, color naming OCD-related, but not PD-related, words correlated with increased activation of frontal-striatal and temporal regions, although performance was unimpaired. In contrast, in PD, increased frontal-striatal involvement was found during color naming both OCD-related and panic-related words. In PD, color naming panic-related words was slowed and correlated with increased activation of the right amygdala and hippocampus. Patients with hypochondriasis showed a similar activation pattern to patients with PD. CONCLUSIONS Our results support the hypothesis of increased distractibility for irrelevant information in patients with OCD, PD, and hypochondriasis associated with frontal-striatal and limbic involvement compared with controls. Although patients with OCD did not display an attentional bias in behavior relative to controls, there was a clear, specific neural response during color naming OCD-related words, involving mainly ventral brain regions. In contrast, generalized emotional interference effects were found in PD and hypochondriasis, involving ventral and widespread dorsal brain regions, reflecting not only unconscious emotional stimulus processing but also increased cognitive elaboration.

[1]  Henk J Groenewegen,et al.  Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2005, Archives of general psychiatry.

[2]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[3]  Fritz Hohagen,et al.  Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[4]  J. Lachenmeyer,et al.  Anxiety and selective attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2003, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  S. Rauch,et al.  Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  J. Lépine,et al.  Selective attention in social phobia and the moderating effect of a concurrent depressive disorder. , 2003, Behaviour research and therapy.

[7]  E. Hollander,et al.  Obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders , 2003, Dialogues in clinical neuroscience.

[8]  D. Charney,et al.  Neuroanatomical circuits modulating fear and anxiety behaviors , 2003, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica. Supplementum.

[9]  A. Addington,et al.  The identification of OCD-related subgroups based on comorbidity , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[10]  Guillem Massana,et al.  Amygdalar atrophy in panic disorder patients detected by volumetric magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  M. Buonocore,et al.  Brain regions showing increased activation by threat-related words in panic disorder , 2003, Neuroreport.

[12]  Bill Faw Pre-frontal executive committee for perception, working memory, attention, long-term memory, motor control, and thinking: A tutorial review , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  C. Junqué,et al.  Parahippocampal gray matter density in panic disorder: a voxel-based morphometric study. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[14]  Ryouhei Ishii,et al.  Parallel distributed processing neuroimaging in the Stroop task using spatially filtered magnetoencephalography analysis , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[15]  C. Gonsalvez,et al.  Response inhibition deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder , 2002, Psychiatry Research.

[16]  Deborah Yurgelun-Todd,et al.  Stroop Performance in Normal Control Subjects: An fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[17]  R. Heimberg,et al.  Memory biases in the anxiety disorders: current status. , 2002, Clinical psychology review.

[18]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.

[19]  D. Cohen,et al.  Perceptual consequences of an illness-concern induction and its relation to hypochondriacal tendencies. , 2002, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[20]  Mark B. Powers,et al.  Functional MRI changes during panic anticipation and imagery exposure , 2001, Neuroreport.

[21]  S. Rauch,et al.  An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  N. Cohen,et al.  The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  R. Henson,et al.  Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  W. Roth,et al.  The emotional Stroop effect in anxiety disorders: general emotional or disorder specificity? , 2001, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[25]  Steven C.R. Williams,et al.  Time courses of left and right amygdalar responses to fearful facial expressions , 2001, NeuroImage.

[26]  S. Rauch,et al.  Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli , 2001, Neuroreport.

[27]  G. Lohmann,et al.  Color-Word Matching Stroop Task: Separating Interference and Response Conflict , 2001, NeuroImage.

[28]  D. McKay,et al.  Overlapping and distinctive features of hypochondriasis and obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2000, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[29]  N. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal regions play a predominant role in imposing an attentional 'set': evidence from fMRI. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[30]  E. Bullmore,et al.  A differential neural response in obsessive–compulsive disorder patients with washing compared with checking symptoms to disgust , 2000, Psychological Medicine.

[31]  S. Holland,et al.  fMRI of neuronal activation with symptom provocation in unmedicated patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. , 2000, Journal of psychiatric research.

[32]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[33]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  An event-related functional MRI study of the stroop color word interference task. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Differential neural responses to overt and covert presentations of facial expressions of fear and disgust , 2000, NeuroImage.

[35]  K. Carlsson,et al.  Top-down inhibitory modulation of early visual cortex in subjects with specific fear when processing non-conscious phobic stimuli , 2000, NeuroImage.

[36]  J M Gorman,et al.  Neuroanatomical hypothesis of panic disorder, revised. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[37]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[39]  Neal R. Swerdlow,et al.  Visuospatial priming and stroop performance in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[40]  A. Anderson,et al.  An fMRI study of stroop word-color interference: evidence for cingulate subregions subserving multiple distributed attentional systems , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  Lars-Göran Öst,et al.  Preattentive bias for emotional information in panic disorder with agoraphobia. , 1999, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[42]  R. Lydiard,et al.  Brain circuits in panic disorder , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[43]  George Bush,et al.  The emotional counting stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[44]  A L Brody,et al.  Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder , 1998, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[45]  M. Kyrios,et al.  Automatic and strategic processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder: attentional bias, cognitive avoidance or more complex phenomena? , 1998, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[46]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[47]  F. Hohagen,et al.  Cognitive Frontal Lobe Dysfunction in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[48]  S. Paradiso The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life , 1998 .

[49]  D. Perrett,et al.  A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust , 1997, Nature.

[50]  S. Kornblum,et al.  Isolation of Specific Interference Processing in the Stroop Task: PET Activation Studies , 1997, NeuroImage.

[51]  A Baddeley,et al.  The fractionation of working memory. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  Joseph E LeDoux The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life , 1996 .

[53]  R. McNally,et al.  Suppression of the emotional Stroop effect by increased anxiety in patients with social phobia. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[54]  D. Perrett,et al.  A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions , 1996, Nature.

[55]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[56]  J. R. Baker,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[57]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[58]  M. Deiber,et al.  A controlled positron emission tomography study of obsessive and neutral auditory stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder with checking rituals , 1996, Psychiatry Research.

[59]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Interference and Facilitation Effects during Selective Attention: An H2 15O PET Study of Stroop Task Performance , 1995, NeuroImage.

[60]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  P. Cleary,et al.  Hypochondriasis and panic disorder. Boundary and overlap. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[62]  C D Frith,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Obsessive–Compulsive Phenomena , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[63]  P. van Oppen,et al.  Selective processing of emotional information in obsessive compulsive disorder. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[64]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the stroop test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  K. Mogg,et al.  Effects of anxiety and awareness on colour-identification latencies for emotional words. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[66]  K. Mogg,et al.  Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[67]  Edna B. Foa,et al.  Information processing in obsessive—compulsive disorder , 1993, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[68]  J. Mattia,et al.  The revised Stroop color-naming task in social phobics. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[69]  R. McNally,et al.  Cognitive processing of emotional information in panic disorder. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[70]  M. Kozak,et al.  Processing of threat-related information in rape victims. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[71]  R. McNally,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in posttraumatic stress disorder. , 1990, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[72]  A. Syrota,et al.  Obsessive‐compulsive disorder: a clinical, neuropsychological and positron emission tomography study , 1990, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[73]  W. Goodman,et al.  The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. Development, use, and reliability. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[74]  P T Fox,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of a lactate-induced anxiety attack. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[75]  J. Gorman,et al.  A neuroanatomical hypothesis for panic disorder. , 1989, The American journal of psychiatry.

[76]  W. Roth,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in subjects with panic attacks , 1988 .

[77]  F P McKenna,et al.  Colour naming of phobia-related words. , 1986, British journal of psychology.

[78]  D. Amaral,et al.  Amygdalo‐cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis) , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[79]  Peter Herscovitch,et al.  A focal brain abnormality in panic disorder, a severe form of anxiety , 1984, Nature.

[80]  E. Perret The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  I. Pilowsky,et al.  Dimensions of Hypochondriasis , 1967, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[82]  John E. Roberts,et al.  Memory and attention in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: a review. , 2005, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[83]  E. Libman,et al.  Selective attentional bias related to the noticeability aspect of anxiety symptoms in generalized social phobia. , 2003, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[84]  M. Verbraak,et al.  The emotional Stroop: a comparison of panic disorder patients, obsessive-compulsive patients, and normal controls, in two experiments. , 2002, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[85]  E. Foa,et al.  Enhanced Stroop interference for threat in social phobia. , 2002, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[86]  Dean McKay,et al.  Attention bias for disgust. , 2002, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[87]  Lars-Göran Öst,et al.  Attentional and memory bias for emotional information in crime victims with acute posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). , 2002, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[88]  S. Moritz,et al.  Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. , 2002, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[89]  JaneR . Taylor,et al.  Impaired Inhibition of Conditioned Responses Produced by Subchronic Administration of Phencyclidine to Rats , 2001, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[90]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[91]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[92]  A. Beck,et al.  An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[93]  M. Stein,et al.  Hypochondriacal concerns and somatization in panic disorder , 1997, Depression and anxiety.

[94]  N. Alpert,et al.  Probing striatal function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a PET study of implicit sequence learning. , 1997, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[95]  A. Pickering,et al.  Attentional bias in obsessional compulsive disorder. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[96]  P. Emmelkamp,et al.  The structure of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[97]  R. McNally,et al.  Cognitive processing of idiographic emotional information in panic disorder. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[98]  N. Alpert,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[99]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Regional brain activity when selecting a response despite interference: An H2 15O PET study of the stroop and an emotional stroop , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[100]  A Arntz,et al.  Attentional bias and spider phobia: conceptual and clinical issues. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[101]  C. Carter,et al.  Patterns of abnormal processing of emotional information in panic disorder and major depression. , 1992, Psychopathology.

[102]  D. Clark,et al.  Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information? , 1991, Behaviour research and therapy.

[103]  R. McNally,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in panic disorder. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[104]  M. Raichle,et al.  The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[105]  E. Sanavio Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory. , 1988, Behaviour research and therapy.

[106]  D. Clark A cognitive approach to panic. , 1986, Behaviour research and therapy.

[107]  M E Raichle,et al.  The application of positron emission tomography to the study of panic disorder. , 1986, The American journal of psychiatry.

[108]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.

[109]  D. Chambless,et al.  Assessment of fear of fear in agoraphobics: the body sensations questionnaire and the agoraphobic cognitions questionnaire. , 1984, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[110]  S. Rauch,et al.  Human Brain Mapping 6:270–282(1998) � The Counting Stroop: An Interference Task Specialized for Functional Neuroimaging—Validation Study With Functional MRI , 2022 .