Comparison of Force Matching Performance in Conventional and Laparoscopic Force-Based Task

Laparoscopic instruments have limited haptics feedback. Hence, novices tend to exert excessive force which leads to tissue trauma. In laparoscopic surgery, no external information is available on the magnitude of excessive force. Therefore, novices should be trained to accurately perceive their own force output. This study analyzed the force perception of 18 novices in the absence of external information, by comparing the isometric force matching performance of index finger (i.e. used in conventional procedures) in extended arm posture with that of laparoscopic instrument in a force-based probing task. The study also examined the effect of handedness on force perception. A contra-lateral force matching paradigm was employed to analyze the matching performance of the novice subjects. Interestingly, matching error was found to be lower for laparoscopic instrument. An effect of handedness was visible for laparoscopic instrument only. The dominant hand overestimated the forces of non-dominant hand. The results can be used as a performance metric to evaluate the force perception of novices in laparoscopic force skills-training tasks.

[1]  A Cuschieri,et al.  Analysis of errors enacted by surgical trainees during skills training courses. , 2005, Surgery.

[2]  R Anup,et al.  Surgical stress and the gastrointestinal tract. , 2000, The Journal of surgical research.

[3]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Effects of related sensory inputs on motor performances in man studied through changes in perceived heaviness , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  A. Cuschieri,et al.  Psychomotor skills for endoscopic manipulations: differing abilities between right and left-handed individuals. , 1997, Annals of surgery.

[5]  H. Xina,et al.  Laparoscopic surgery , perceptual limitations and force : A review , 2006 .

[6]  S. Wexner,et al.  Adhesion formation after laparoscopic anterior resection in a porcine model: a pilot study. , 1996, Surgical laparoscopy & endoscopy.

[7]  T. Johansen,et al.  The Role of Tactile Feedback in Laparoscopic Surgery , 2006, Surgical laparoscopy, endoscopy & percutaneous techniques.

[8]  L. Way,et al.  Causes and Prevention of Laparoscopic Bile Duct Injuries: Analysis of 252 Cases From a Human Factors and Cognitive Psychology Perspective , 2003, Annals of surgery.

[9]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  D. Stegeman,et al.  A randomized trial of training the non-dominant upper extremity to enhance laparoscopic performance , 2012, Minimally invasive therapy & allied technologies : MITAT : official journal of the Society for Minimally Invasive Therapy.

[11]  Christopher J. Martin,et al.  Grasper trauma during laparoscopic cholecystectomy. , 2000, The Australian and New Zealand journal of surgery.

[12]  L. A. Jones Matching Forces: Constant Errors and Differential Thresholds , 1989, Perception.

[13]  A. Cuschieri,et al.  Errors enacted during endoscopic surgery--a human reliability analysis. , 1998, Applied ergonomics.

[14]  T. Grantcharov,et al.  Impact of hand dominance, gender, and experience with computer games on performance in virtual reality laparoscopy , 2003, Surgical Endoscopy And Other Interventional Techniques.

[15]  S. Chauvin,et al.  Hand-Eye Dominance and Depth Perception Effects in Performance on a Basic Laparoscopic Skills Set , 2010, JSLS : Journal of the Society of Laparoendoscopic Surgeons.

[16]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Changes in motor commands, as shown by changes in perceived heaviness, during partial curarization and peripheral anaesthesia in man , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  Stephan Riek,et al.  Central and peripheral mediation of human force sensation following eccentric or concentric contractions , 2002, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  Simon C Gandevia,et al.  Overestimation of force during matching of externally generated forces , 2011, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Perceptual constancy and the perceived magnitude of muscle forces , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  A. Cuschieri,et al.  Extent of innate dexterity and ambidexterity across handedness and gender: Implications for training in laparoscopic surgery , 2007, Surgical Endoscopy.

[21]  T. Krummel,et al.  Tactile feedback is present during minimally invasive surgery. , 1999, Journal of the American College of Surgeons.

[22]  Caroline G. L. Cao,et al.  Effects of Vision and Friction on Haptic Perception , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[23]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Two Eyes for an Eye: The Neuroscience of Force Escalation , 2003, Science.

[24]  G. Picod,et al.  What can the operator actually feel when performing a laparoscopy? , 2003, Surgical Endoscopy And Other Interventional Techniques.

[25]  C. T. Leonard,et al.  Finger force perception during ipsilateral and contralateral force matching tasks , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  B. Hannaford,et al.  Force-feedback grasper helps restore sense of touch in minimally invasive surgery , 1999, Journal of Gastrointestinal Surgery.

[27]  W. Sjoerdsma,et al.  Sensitivity of laparoscopic dissectors , 1999, Surgical Endoscopy.

[28]  B. Bigland-ritchie,et al.  Sensation of static force in muscles of different length , 1979, Experimental Neurology.