Neurobiological Models of Visuospatial Cognition in Children With Williams Syndrome: Measures of Dorsal-Stream and Frontal Function

We examine hypotheses for the neural basis of the profile of visual cognition in young children with Williams syndrome (WS). These are: (a) that it is a consequence of anomalies in sensory visual processing, (b) that it is a deficit of the dorsal relative to the ventral cortical stream, (c) that it reflects deficit of frontal function, in particular of frontoparietal interaction, and (d) that it is related to impaired function in the right hemisphere relative to the left. The tests reported here are particularly relevant to hypotheses 2 and 3. They form part of a more extensive program of investigating visual, visuospatial, and cognitive function in large group of children with WS children, aged 8 months to 15 years. To compare performance across tests, avoiding floor and ceiling effects, we have measured performance in children with WS in terms of the "age equivalence" for typically developing children. In this article the relation between dorsal and ventral function is tested by motion and form coherence thresholds, respectively. We confirm the presence of a subgroup of children with WS who perform particularly poorly on the motion (dorsal) task. However, such performance is also characteristic of normally developing children up to 5 years; thus the WS performance may reflect an overall persisting immaturity of visuospatial processing that is particularly evident in the dorsal stream. Looking at the performance on the global coherence tasks of the entire WS group, we find that there is also a subgroup who have both high form and motion coherence thresholds, relative to the performance of children of the same chronological age and verbal age on the British Picture Vocabulary Scale, suggesting a more general global processing deficit.

[1]  J Wattam-Bell,et al.  Changes in Infants' Ability to Switch Visual Attention in the First Three Months of Life , 1992, Perception.

[2]  T. L. Jernigan,et al.  Specific neurobehavioral profile of Williams' syndrome is associated with neocerebellar hemispheric preservation , 1992, Neurology.

[3]  U Bellugi,et al.  Cytoarchitectonic anomalies in a genetically based disorder: Williams syndrome. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[4]  A. Diamond,et al.  The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1 2 –7 years old on a stroop- like day-night test , 1994, Cognition.

[5]  James A. Russell,et al.  Autistic children's difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. , 1993 .

[6]  D. Bishop Comprehension in Developmental Language Disorders , 1979 .

[7]  B. Hood Gravity rules for 2- to 4-year olds? , 1995 .

[8]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Possible blindsight in infants lacking one cerebral hemisphere , 1992, Nature.

[9]  J. Duncan,et al.  Intelligence and the Frontal Lobe: The Organization of Goal-Directed Behavior , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  T Shallice,et al.  The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  J. Atkinson,et al.  INFANTS CONTROL OF FIXATION SHIFTS WITH SINGLE AND COMPETING TARGETS - MECHANISMS OF SHIFTING ATTENTION , 1988 .

[12]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Visual and visuospatial development in young children with Williams syndrome , 2001, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[13]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Visual acuity measures in a sample of Williams' syndrome , 1997 .

[14]  P. Jarvis,et al.  Relationships between parenting stress and developmental functioning among 2-year-olds , 1994 .

[15]  A. Deckel Hemispheric control of territorial aggression in Anolis carolinensis: effects of mild stress. , 1998, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[16]  R. Turner,et al.  Brain Areas Differentially Activated by Coherent Visual Motion and Dynamic Noise , 1998, NeuroImage.

[17]  J. Atkinson,et al.  EARLY DEVELOPMENT OF THE CONTROL OF VISUAL-ATTENTION , 1985 .

[18]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  VISUAL AND VISUOSPATIAL DEVELOPMENT IN YOUNG WILLIAMS-SYNDROME CHILDREN , 1995 .

[19]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Form and motion coherence: Is there dorsal stream vulnerability in development , 1999 .

[20]  Joan Stiles,et al.  Spatial grouping activity in young children with congenital right or left hemisphere brain injury , 1991, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  The Developing Visual Brain , 2000 .

[22]  Terry L. Jernigan,et al.  Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. , 1994 .

[23]  Y Agid,et al.  Cortical control of reflexive visually-guided saccades. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  C. Mervis,et al.  Drawing by children with Williams syndrome: A developmental perspective , 1997 .

[25]  A. Karmiloff-Smith Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Y. Agid,et al.  Lateral visually-guided saccades in progressive supranuclear palsy. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Verbal and nonverbal abilities in the Williams syndrome phenotype: evidence for diverging developmental trajectories. , 1998, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[28]  D. Rose,et al.  Models of the visual cortex , 1985 .

[29]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Development of Orientation Discrimination in Infancy , 1988, Perception.

[30]  J. Stiles-Davis Developmental change in young children's spatial grouping activity. , 1988 .

[31]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  C. Baker,et al.  Residual motion perception in a "motion-blind" patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. , 2005, American journal of medical genetics. Supplement.

[34]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Seeing either the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders : implications for brain function , 1994 .

[36]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[37]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal cortex: from sight to action , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[39]  Carolyn B. Mervis,et al.  Contrasting Patterns of Cognitive Abilities of 9- and 10-Year-Olds With Williams Syndrome or Down Syndrome , 1999 .

[40]  J. Russell,et al.  The execution of arbitrary procedures by children with autism , 2001, Development and Psychopathology.

[41]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants , 1994, Vision Research.

[42]  U Bellugi,et al.  Cerebral morphologic distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. , 1993, Archives of neurology.

[43]  O Braddick,et al.  Basal ganglia damage and impaired visual function in the newborn infant , 1997, Archives of disease in childhood. Fetal and neonatal edition.

[44]  U Bellugi,et al.  Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. , 1990, Archives of neurology.

[45]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Selectivity for polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Science.

[46]  R. Turner,et al.  Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain , 2000, Current Biology.

[47]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Brain biochemistry in Williams syndrome , 1998, Neurology.

[48]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[49]  O. Braddick,et al.  A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams' syndrome , 1997, Neuroreport.

[50]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[51]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Basal ganglia damage in the newborn infant as a predictor of impaired visual function , 1997 .

[52]  O Braddick,et al.  Visual function and perinatal focal cerebral infarction. , 1996, Archives of disease in childhood. Fetal and neonatal edition.

[53]  C. Mervis,et al.  Global Spatial Organization by Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1999 .

[54]  V. Volterra,et al.  Children with Williams syndrome : Is there a single neuropsychological profile? , 1999 .

[55]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  INFANT CORTICAL MECHANISMS CONTROLLING OKN, SACCADIC SHIFTS AND MOTION PROCESSING , 1993 .

[56]  T. Robbins Dissociating executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[57]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Chiari I malformation in asymptomatic young children with Williams syndrome: clinical and MRI study. , 1997, European journal of paediatric neurology : EJPN : official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society.

[58]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[59]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[60]  A. Diamond,et al.  Evidence for the importance of dopamine for prefrontal cortex functions early in life. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.