Precision of speed discrimination and smooth pursuit eye movements

Several studies have shown that the precision of smooth pursuit eye speed can match perceptual speed discrimination thresholds during the steady-state phase of pursuit [Kowler, E., & McKee, S. (1987). Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity. Vision Research, 27, 993-1015; Gegenfurtner, K., Xing, D., Scott, B., & Hawken, M. (2003). A comparison of pursuit eye movement and perceptual performance in speed discrimination. Journal of Vision, 3, 865-876]. Recently, Osborne et al. [Osborne, L. C., Lisberger, S. G., & Bialek, W. (2005). A sensory source for motor variation. Nature, 437, 412-416; Osborne, L. C., Hohl, S. S., Bialek, W., & Lisberger S. G. (2007). Time course of precision in smooth-pursuit eye movements of monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 2987-2998] claimed that pursuit precision during the initiation phase of pursuit also matches the sensory variability, implying that there is no motor noise added during pursuit initiation. However, these results were derived from a comparison of monkey pursuit data to human perceptual data from the literature, which were obtained with different stimuli. To directly compare precision for perception and pursuit, we measured pursuit and perceptual variability in the same human observers using the same stimuli. Subjects had to pursue a Gaussian blob in a step-ramp paradigm and give speed judgments on the same or in different trials. Speed discrimination thresholds were determined for different presentation durations. The analysis of pursuit precision was performed for short intervals containing the initiation period only and also for longer intervals including steady-state pursuit. In agreement with published studies, we found that the Weber fractions for psychophysical speed discrimination were fairly constant for different presentation durations, even for the shortest presentation duration of 150ms. Pursuit variability was 3-4 times as high for the analysis interval (300ms) containing the open-loop phase only. For pursuit analysis intervals of 400-500ms, pursuit variability approached perceptual variability. Our results show that, for the stimuli we used, the motor system contributes at least 50% to the total variability of smooth pursuit eye movements during the initiation phase.

[1]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Effects of contrast on smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[2]  Mark M. Churchland,et al.  Supplemental Data A Central Source of Movement Variability , 2006 .

[3]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  Tuning for Spatiotemporal Frequency and Speed in Directionally Selective Neurons of Macaque Striate Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Computational models of cortical visual processing. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[7]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  A Logarithmic, Scale-Invariant Representation of Speed in Macaque Middle Temporal Area Accounts for Speed Discrimination Performance , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  W. Newsome,et al.  Correlation between Speed Perception and Neural Activity in the Middle Temporal Visual Area , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  R. J. Beers The Sources of Variability in Saccadic Eye Movements , 2007 .

[10]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  M J Hawken,et al.  Pursuit eye movements to second-order motion targets. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[12]  C. W. G Clifford,et al.  Fundamental mechanisms of visual motion detection: models, cells and functions , 2002, Progress in Neurobiology.

[13]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  Motion grouping impairs speed discrimination , 2006, Vision Research.

[14]  W. Bialek,et al.  Time Course of Information about Motion Direction in Visual Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Brian H Scott,et al.  A comparison of pursuit eye movement and perceptual performance in speed discrimination. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[16]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning , 1998, Nature.

[17]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Visual motion processing for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  M. Hawken,et al.  Smooth pursuit eye movements to isoluminant targets. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  L S Stone,et al.  Motion coherence affects human perception and pursuit similarly , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[20]  S. McKee,et al.  Precise velocity discrimination despite random variations in temporal frequency and contrast , 1986, Vision Research.

[21]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Reconstruction of Target Speed for the Guidance of Pursuit Eye Movements , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  A comparison of localization judgments and pointing precision. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[23]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[25]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Shared motion signals for human perceptual decisions and oculomotor actions. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[26]  Graham J Hole,et al.  I can't recognize your face but I can recognize its movement , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[27]  William Bialek,et al.  Time Course of Precision in Smooth-Pursuit Eye Movements of Monkeys , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Braun,et al.  Motion aftereffect elicits smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[29]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity , 1987, Vision Research.

[30]  W. Bialek,et al.  A sensory source for motor variation , 2005, Nature.

[31]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[32]  Katherine I. Nagel,et al.  Cortical Mechanisms of Smooth Eye Movements Revealed by Dynamic Covariations of Neural and Behavioral Responses , 2008, Neuron.

[33]  S. McKee,et al.  Visual information throughout a reach determines endpoint precision , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—III. Guessing unpredictable target displacements , 1981, Vision Research.

[36]  H. D. Crane,et al.  Accurate three-dimensional eyetracker. , 1978, Applied optics.

[37]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Variation, Signal, and Noise in Cerebellar Sensory–Motor Processing for Smooth-Pursuit Eye Movements , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  K. Shenoy,et al.  A Central Source of Movement Variability , 2006, Neuron.

[39]  R Sekuler,et al.  Motion Perception: A Modern View of Wertheimer's 1912 Monograph , 1996, Perception.

[40]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Tracking with the mind’s eye , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[41]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Psychophysical and saccadic information about direction for briefly presented visual targets , 1992, Vision Research.

[42]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Smooth pursuit eye movements under open-loop and closed-loop conditions , 1983, Vision Research.

[43]  Brent R. Beutter,et al.  Human motion perception and smooth eye movements slow similar directional biases for elongated apertures , 1998, Vision Research.

[44]  Maninder K. Kahlon,et al.  Visual Motion Analysis for Pursuit Eye Movements in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Chantal Delon-Martin,et al.  Timing of interactions across the visual field in the human cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[46]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The role of execution noise in movement variability. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  J Lorenceau,et al.  Visual Motion Integration for Perception and Pursuit , 2000, Perception.

[48]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[49]  Suzanne P. McKee,et al.  Initial visual information determines endpoint precision for rapid pointing , 2006, Vision Research.

[50]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[51]  D. Bradley,et al.  Neural population code for fine perceptual decisions in area MT , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.