Gender Stereotypes and Women's Reports of Liking and Ability in Traditionally Masculine and Feminine Occupations

Gender stereotypes were examined for their causal influence on women's reported liking for and perceived ability to succeed in traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. One hundred twenty-one women were assigned to either a gender-stereotype activation or filler task and then completed measures of liking for, and perceived ability to succeed in, traditionally masculine and feminine occupations. Strongly gender-identified women showed significantly greater liking for feminine occupations in the stereotype-activation condition than in the control condition. However, more weakly identified women did not show the same effect. In contrast, women weak in gender identification reported an increase in perceived ability for feminine occupations when stereotypes were activated than in the control condition. Activating gender stereotypes did not shift reported liking or perceived ability in traditionally masculine occupations. These results demonstrate the theoretical and practical importance of gender stereotypes on women's career-related attitudes.

[1]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender-Stereotypic Images of Occupations Correspond to the Sex Segregation of Employment , 1999 .

[2]  B. Levy Handwriting as a reflection of aging self-stereotypes. , 2000 .

[3]  J. Eccles,et al.  Gender Role Stereotypes, Expectancy Effects, and Parents' Socialization of Gender Differences , 1990 .

[4]  C. Steele A threat in the air. How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[5]  Jacquelynne S. Eccles,et al.  Linking gender to educational, occupational, and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. , 1999 .

[6]  Nalini Ambady,et al.  “Math is Hard!” The effect of gender priming on women’s attitudes , 2006 .

[7]  N. Ambady,et al.  Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity Salience and Shifts in Quantitative Performance , 1999 .

[8]  Larry E. Toothaker,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .

[9]  B. Levy,et al.  Mind Matters: Cognitive and Physical Effects of Aging Self-stereotypes Internalization of Aging Stereotypes , 2022 .

[10]  Marianne LaFrance,et al.  No Laughing Matter: Women's Verbal and Nonverbal Reactions to Sexist Humor , 1998 .

[11]  D. Marx,et al.  Female Role Models: Protecting Women’s Math Test Performance , 2002 .

[12]  J. Croizet,et al.  Stereotype Threat: Are Lower Status and History of Stigmatization Preconditions of Stereotype Threat? , 2000 .

[13]  Linda C. Schmidt,et al.  Social Cognitive Predictors of Academic Interests and Goals in Engineering: Utility for Women and Students at Historically Black Universities. , 2005 .

[14]  J. Eccles Gender Roles and Women's Achievement-Related Decisions , 1987 .

[15]  Jacquelynne S. Eccles,et al.  Understanding Women's Educational And Occupational Choices: Applying the Eccles et al. Model of Achievement-Related Choices , 1994 .

[16]  Jacqueline H. Watts,et al.  IV. Can't Take a Joke? Humour as Resistance, Refuge and Exclusion in a Highly Gendered Workplace , 2007 .

[17]  Daniel J. Bauer,et al.  Simple Intercepts, Simple Slopes, and Regions of Significance in MLR 2-Way Interactions , 2004 .

[18]  Denise Sekaquaptewa,et al.  Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance , 2003 .

[19]  Toni Schmader,et al.  Gender Identification Moderates Stereotype Threat Effects on Women's Math Performance ☆ ☆☆ ★ , 2002 .

[20]  C. Chiu,et al.  Stereotyping and Self-Presentation: Effects of Gender Stereotype Activation , 1998 .

[21]  Robert W. Lent,et al.  Relation of Contextual Supports and Barriers to Choice Behavior in Engineering Majors: Test of Alternative Social Cognitive Models. , 2003 .

[22]  Bryan L Bonner,et al.  Motivated self-stereotyping: heightened assimilation and differentiation needs result in increased levels of positive and negative self-stereotyping. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  M. Biernat,et al.  Selective self-stereotyping. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  B. Levy Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Richard D. Harvey,et al.  A Q-Methodological Study of Women's Subjective Perspectives on Mathematics , 2003 .

[26]  Christian S. Crandall,et al.  Stereotype Threat and Arousal: Effects on Women's Math Performance , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[27]  D. Oswald,et al.  The Content and Function of Gender Self-stereotypes: An Exploratory Investigation , 2006 .

[28]  Kevin J. Williams,et al.  The Effects of Stereotype Threat and Double-Minority Status on the Test Performance of Latino Women , 2002 .

[29]  Johannes Keller,et al.  Blatant Stereotype Threat and Women's Math Performance: Self-Handicapping as a Strategic Means to Cope with Obtrusive Negative Performance Expectations , 2002 .

[30]  Robert W. Lent,et al.  Contextual supports and barriers to career choice: A social cognitive analysis. , 2000 .

[31]  C. G. Lord,et al.  Alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat through salience of group achievements , 2003 .

[32]  Richard D. Harvey,et al.  Hostile environments, stereotype threat, and math performance among undergraduate women , 2000 .

[33]  Robert W. Lent,et al.  The role of contextual supports and barriers in the choice of math/science educational options: A test of social cognitive hypotheses , 2001 .

[34]  Joshua Aronson,et al.  Shades of Threat: Racial Identity as a Moderator of Stereotype Threat , 2006 .

[35]  Steven D. Brown,et al.  Toward a Unifying Social Cognitive Theory of Career and Academic Interest, Choice, and Performance , 1994 .

[36]  M. L. Hummert,et al.  Memory Performance of Late Middle–Aged Adults: Contrasting Self–Stereotyping and Stereotype Threat Accounts of Assimilation to Age Stereotypes , 2006 .

[37]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Math = male, me = female, therefore math ≠ me. , 2002 .

[38]  Michael Johns,et al.  The Costs of Accepting Gender Differences: The Role of Stereotype Endorsement in Women's Experience in the Math Domain , 2004 .

[39]  Marilynn B. Brewer,et al.  Assimilation and differentiation needs as motivational determinants of perceived in-group and out-group homogeneity , 2001 .

[40]  G. Henly,et al.  Occupational Sextype and Sex Differences in Vocational Preference-Measured Interest Relationships , 1998 .

[41]  M. Shih,et al.  Domain-specific effects of stereotypes on performance , 2006 .

[42]  S. Spencer,et al.  Stereotype Threat and Women's Math Performance , 1999 .

[43]  Shaughnessy,et al.  Efficacy Expectations and Vocational Interests as Mediators between Sex and Choice of Math/Science College Majors: A Longitudinal Study , 1996, Journal of vocational behavior.

[44]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  A Threatening Intellectual Environment: Why Females Are Susceptible to Experiencing Problem-Solving Deficits in the Presence of Males , 2000, Psychological science.

[45]  K. Fujita,et al.  Stereotype performance boosts: the impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  Joshua Aronson,et al.  Stereotype Threat and the Gender Gap in Political Knowledge , 2006 .

[47]  Joshua Aronson,et al.  Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning , 2006 .

[48]  C. Steele,et al.  Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  Rosalind C. Barnett,et al.  Learning in a Man's World: Examining the Perceptions of Undergraduate Women in Male-Dominated Academic Areas , 2002 .