Reduced sensitivity to neutral feedback versus negative feedback in subjects with mild depression: Evidence from event-related potentials study

Many previous event-related potential (ERP) studies have linked the feedback related negativity (FRN) component with medial frontal cortex processing and associated this component with depression. Few if any studies have investigated the processing of neutral feedback in mildly depressive subjects in the normal population. Two experiments compared brain responses to neutral feedback with behavioral performance in mildly depressed subjects who scored highly on the Beck Depression Inventory (high BDI) and a control group with lower BDI scores (low BDI). In the first study, the FRN component was recorded when neutral, negative or positive feedback was pseudo-randomly delivered to the two groups in a time estimation task. In the second study, real feedback was provided to the two groups in the same task in order to measure their actual accuracy of performance. The results of experiment one (Exp. 1) revealed that a larger FRN effect was elicited by neutral feedback than by negative feedback in the low BDI group, but no significant difference was found between neutral condition and negative condition in the High BDI group. The present findings demonstrated that depressive tendencies influence the processing of neutral feedback in medial frontal cortex. The FRN effect may work as a helpful index for investigating cognitive bias in depression in future studies.

[1]  Thomas D. Sambrook,et al.  A neural reward prediction error revealed by a meta-analysis of ERPs using great grand averages. , 2015, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  Action monitoring and depressive symptom reduction in major depressive disorder. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[3]  Deborah Talmi,et al.  The Feedback-Related Negativity Signals Salience Prediction Errors, Not Reward Prediction Errors , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Responsibility modulates neural mechanisms of outcome processing: an ERP study. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Knowing good from bad: differential activation of human cortical areas by positive and negative outcomes , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[7]  Doreen M. Olvet,et al.  Depression symptom severity and error-related brain activity , 2010, Psychiatry Research.

[8]  Enrico Simonotto,et al.  Limbic over-activity in depression during preserved performance on the n-back task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[9]  R. Baker,et al.  When is an error not a prediction error? An electrophysiological investigation , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  D. Tucker,et al.  Frontolimbic response to negative feedback in clinical depression. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[12]  Wouter Hulstijn,et al.  Action monitoring in major depressive disorder with psychomotor retardation , 2008, Cortex.

[13]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  The good, the bad and the neutral: Electrophysiological responses to feedback stimuli , 2006, Brain Research.

[14]  Yue Ge,et al.  Anxiety and outcome evaluation: The good, the bad and the ambiguous , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[15]  Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin,et al.  Midbrain Dopamine Neurons Signal Preference for Advance Information about Upcoming Rewards , 2009, Neuron.

[16]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  The Devil You Know , 2008, Psychological science.

[17]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Errors in reward prediction are re£ected in the event-related brain potential , 2003 .

[18]  H. Mayberg,et al.  Stuck in a rut: rethinking depression and its treatment , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Paul A Pilkonis,et al.  Neuroticism and affective instability: the same or different? , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[21]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  Learning to Become an Expert: Reinforcement Learning and the Acquisition of Perceptual Expertise , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[23]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and functional significance , 2004, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[24]  A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Brain potentials related to self-generated and external information used for performance monitoring , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[26]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[27]  A. Holmes,et al.  Enhanced negative feedback responses in remitted depression , 2008, Neuroreport.

[28]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Drug-free patients with major depression show an increased electrophysiological response to valid and invalid feedback , 2011, Psychological Medicine.

[29]  Avram J Holmes,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[30]  Uta Sailer,et al.  Manipulation of feedback expectancy and valence induces negative and positive reward prediction error signals manifest in event-related brain potentials. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[31]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Cortical electrophysiological network dynamics of feedback learning , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[33]  Tobias U. Hauser,et al.  The feedback-related negativity (FRN) revisited: New insights into the localization, meaning and network organization , 2014, NeuroImage.

[34]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[35]  Sander Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Mediofrontal negativities in the absence of responding. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[36]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. , 2005, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[37]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The Neural Basis of Responsibility Attribution in Decision-Making , 2013, PloS one.

[39]  W. Schultz Updating dopamine reward signals , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[40]  Tingyong Feng,et al.  Feedback-related negativity effects vanished with false or monetary loss choice , 2009, Neuroreport.

[41]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Learning from experience: Event-related potential correlates of reward processing, neural adaptation, and behavioral choice , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[42]  Sidney J. Segalowitz,et al.  Performance monitoring and the medial prefrontal cortex: a review of individual differences and context effects as a window on self-regulation , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[43]  Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin,et al.  Dopamine in Motivational Control: Rewarding, Aversive, and Alerting , 2010, Neuron.

[44]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Feedback-related negativity observed in rodent anterior cingulate cortex , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[45]  Qiang Liu,et al.  The influence of the diffusion of responsibility effect on outcome evaluations: Electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[46]  G. Hajcak,et al.  Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: Evidence from event-related potentials , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[47]  Adrian G. Fischer,et al.  Neural mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  The effect of erroneous responses on response monitoring in patients with major depressive disorder: a study with event-related potentials. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[49]  David Goodman,et al.  Performance Monitoring in the Anterior Cingulate is Not All Error Related: Expectancy Deviation and the Representation of Action-Outcome Associations , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.