Control of Rapid Arm Movements When Target Position Is Altered During Saccadic Suppression.

This experiment examined whether rapid arm movements can be corrected in response to a change in target position that occurs just prior to movement onset, during saccadic suppression of displacement. Because the threshold of retinal input reaches its highest magnitude at that time, displacement of the visual target of a saccade is not perceived. Subjects (N = 6) were instructed to perform very rapid arm movements toward visual targets located 16, 20, and 24 degrees from midline (on average, movement time was 208 ms). On some trials the 20 degrees target was displaced 4 degrees either to the right or to the left during saccadic suppression. For double-step trials, arm movements did not deviate from their original trajectory. Movement endpoints and movement structure (i.e., velocity-and acceleration-time profiles) were similar whether or not target displacements occurred, showing the failure of proprioceptive signals or internal feedback loops to correct the arm trajectory. Following this movement, terminal spatially oriented movements corrected the direction of the initial movement (as compared with the single-step control trials) when the target eccentricity decreased by 4 degrees. Subjects were unaware of these spatial corrections. Therefore, spatial corrections of hand position were driven by the goal level of the task, which was updated by oculomotor corrective responses when a target shift occurred.

[1]  C. Redon,et al.  Proprioceptive control of goal-directed movements in man, studied by means of vibratory muscle tendon stimulation. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  H. Honda,et al.  Functional between-Hand Differences and Outflow Eye Position Information , 1984, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[3]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  The mapping of visual space is a function of the structure of the visual field , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[4]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[5]  J. Vercher,et al.  The role of ocular muscle proprioception in visual localization of targets. , 1990, Science.

[6]  L. Festinger,et al.  Some observations concerning saccadic eye movements. , 1973, Vision research.

[7]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Saccadic Suppression of Displacement is Strongest in Central Vision , 1990, Perception.

[8]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979 .

[9]  M Crommelinck,et al.  Interactions between goal-directed eye and arm movements: arguments for an interdependent motor control. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  J J Denier van der Gon,et al.  Mechanisms underlying accuracy in fast goal-directed arm movements in man. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[11]  C Ghez,et al.  Gradual specification of response amplitude in human tracking performance. , 1989, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[12]  L. Stark,et al.  Ocular proprioception and efference copy in registering visual direction , 1991, Vision Research.

[13]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Control of multimovement coordination: sensorimotor mechanisms in speech motor programming. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  C. Bard,et al.  The Attentional Cost of Amplitude and Directional Requirements When Pointing to Targets , 1994 .

[15]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Corrective saccades: dependence on retinal reafferent signals , 1975, Vision Research.

[16]  H Honda,et al.  The extraretinal signal from the pursuit-eye-movement system: Its role in the perceptual and the egocentric localization systems , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  W. Wolf,et al.  Corrective saccades: Effect of shifting the saccade goal , 1982, Vision Research.

[18]  L. Stark,et al.  Role of corollary discharge in space constancy , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  R. Baloh,et al.  Quantitative measurement of saccade amplitude, duration, and velocity , 1975, Neurology.

[20]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Michelle Fleury,et al.  Different Patterns in Aiming Accuracy for Head-Movers and Non-Head Movers , 1992 .

[22]  M M Smyth,et al.  Vision and proprioception in simple catching. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  R. L. Hou,et al.  Processing of direction and magnitude by the saccadic eye-movement system , 1979, Vision Research.

[24]  Ronald G. Marteniuk,et al.  The Role of Eye and Head Positions in Slow Movement Execution , 1978 .

[25]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Representation of visuomotor space in the parietal lobe of the monkey. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[26]  C Ghez,et al.  Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[27]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[28]  B. Alstermark,et al.  Visually guided switching of forelimb target reaching in cats. , 1984, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[29]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Modification of trajectory of a pointing movement in response to a change in target location. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  E. Matin Saccadic suppression: a review and an analysis. , 1974, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  H Hatze,et al.  The use of optimally regularized Fourier series for estimating higher-order derivatives of noisy biomechanical data. , 1981, Journal of biomechanics.

[32]  John M. Foley,et al.  Visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues , 1972 .

[33]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  Virginia A. Diggles,et al.  Rapid error correction during human arm movements: evidence for central monitoring. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.