Chronic Stress in Dogs Subjected to Social and Spatial Restriction. I. Behavioral Responses

Six weeks of social and spatial restriction were used as a model to induce chronic stress in Beagles. Behavioral and physiological measurements were performed during a period of enriched spacious outdoor housing in groups (GH) and during a subsequent period of solitary housing in small indoor kennels (IH). Behavioral parameters that may indicate chronic stress in dogs are reported. During IH, the dogs showed significantly (comparison-wise error rate <0.05) lower postures than during GH. IH induced enduring increments in frequencies of autogrooming, paw lifting, and vocalizing, and was associated with incidents of coprophagy and repetitive behavior. So far, we interpret the behavioral changes as signs of chronic stress. Relatively low levels of walking, digging, intentions to change from one state of locomotion to another, and increments in circling are conceived as obvious adaptations to the specific features of the IH system. By challenging the dogs outside their home kennel we tested whether the dogs' coping abilities were affected by IH. Dogs that were challenged were introduced into a novel environment, given the opportunity to escape from their home kennel, restrained, walked down an unfamiliar corridor, presented a novel object, exposed to loud noise, given food, or confronted with a conspecific. During IH, challenged dogs exhibited higher postures, showed more tail wagging, nosing, circling, urinating, and defecating, and changed more often from one state of locomotion (or posture) to another than during GH. These behavioral changes were observed across the different types of challenges, with the exception of the noise administration test. In the presence of conspecifics, the socially and spatially restricted male dogs behaved more dominantly and aggressive than during the time that they were kept in groups. Such behavior manifested as increased performances of raised hairs, growling, paw laying, and standing over. Both sexes showed increases in paw lifting, body shaking, ambivalent postures, intentions to change from one state of locomotion to another, and trembling in any of the challenges, excluding the walking down the corridor test. In short, during a variety of challenges, socially and spatially restricted dogs exhibited a heightened state of aggression, excitement, and uncertainty. Behavioral differences between dogs that had experienced pleasant and bad weather conditions during GH, suggested that "pleasant-weather individuals" had experienced early stress during the control period, and, as a result, responded to the subsequent period of IH differently. Regardless of the housing conditions, challenged bitches showed stronger indications of acute stress than male dogs. Gender did not affect the chronic stress responses to social and spatial restriction. A low posture and increased auto-grooming, paw lifting, vocalizing, repetitive behavior, and coprophagy may indicate chronic stress in dogs, and as such, can help to identify poor welfare. When challenged, chronically stressed dogs may show increased excitement, aggression, and uncertainty, but the nonspecificity of such emotional behavior will complicate its practical use with regard to the assessment of stress.

[1]  R. Solomon,et al.  Traumatic avoidance learning: Acquisition in normal dogs. , 1953 .

[2]  C. Tipton,et al.  A submaximal test for dogs: evaluation of effects of training, detraining, and cage confinement. , 1974, Journal of applied physiology.

[3]  F. Garnier,et al.  Adrenal cortical response in clinically normal dogs before and after adaptation to a housing environment , 1990, Laboratory animals.

[4]  K. Houpt Ingestive behavior problems of dogs and cats. , 1982, The Veterinary clinics of North America. Small animal practice.

[5]  Trevor B. Poole,et al.  Correlates of pen size and housing conditions on the behaviour of kennelled dogs , 1992 .

[6]  S. A. Corson,et al.  Constitutional Differences in Physiologic Adaptation to Stress and Distress , 1976 .

[7]  Bonne Beerda,et al.  Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs , 1998 .

[8]  T. Uhde,et al.  24-Hour motor activity in genetically nervous and normal pointer dogs , 1988, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  J. P. Scott,et al.  The development of emotional distress reactions to separation, in puppies. , 1961, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[10]  P. A. Mattis,et al.  Effect of cage size on patterns of activity and health of beagle dogs. , 1977, Laboratory animal science.

[11]  Newton Wm An evaluation of the effects of various degrees of long-term confinement on adult beagle dogs. , 1972 .

[12]  J. Mateo,et al.  Influence of housing conditions on beagle behaviour , 1992 .