Sexual Selection and Information Content in Gibbon Song Bouts

Male and female gibbons (Hylobates) produce sexually dimorphic song bouts which appear to serve to guard mates and defend territories respectively. On the basis of differences in both the familiarity and the costs of conflict between sender and receiver, males but not females are hypothesized to use high-energy assessment signals to advertise resource-holding potential. This hypothesis is tested by examining the evidence for differential energy constraints in the production of male and female song bouts across 21 gibbon populations. The results indicate that song performance is reduced when the availability of high-energy foods is reduced and that this effect is greatest in males. Male song bout frequency also declines in populations where the energetic costs of thermoregulation are likely to be greater (i.e. in high-latitude populations). Females do not show this pattern. Females appear to perform the minimum number of songs per bout required for signal transmission and may perform bouts less often when more songs are required. In contrast, males sing for longer periods the more frequently bouts are performed. Overall, these results support the hypothesis that the song bouts of males but not females act as assessment signals. Observational and experimental studies which would provide a more powerful test of this hypothesis are briefly outlined.

[1]  E. H. Haimoff,et al.  Loud Calls of the Gibbon (Hylobates Lar): Repertoire, Organisation and Context , 1984 .

[2]  M. Ryan Energy, calling, and selection , 1988 .

[3]  A. Møller Why Mated Songbirds Sing So Much: Mate Guarding and Male Announcement of Mate Fertility Status , 1991, The American Naturalist.

[4]  R. Palombit Lethal territorial aggression in a white‐handed gibbon , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[5]  R R Tenaza,et al.  Songs, choruses and countersinging of Kloss' gibbons (Hylobates klossii) in Siberut Island, Indonesia. , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.

[6]  J. Mitani Territoriality and monogamy among agile gibbons (Hylobates agilis) , 1987, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[7]  T. Clutton‐Brock,et al.  THE ROARING OF RED DEER AND THE EVOLUTION OF HONEST ADVERTISEMENT , 1979 .

[8]  C. P. Schaik Phenological changes in a Sumatran rain forest , 1986, Journal of Tropical Ecology.

[9]  M. Zuk,et al.  Energy cost of an avian vocal display: crowing in red junglefowl , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  J. Mitani Gibbon Song Duets and Intergroup Spacing , 1985 .

[11]  A. Whitten A Numerical Analysis of Tropical Rain Forest, Using Floristic and Structural Data, and Its Application to an Analysis of Gibbon Ranging Behaviour , 1982 .

[12]  K. McComb Female choice for high roaring rates in red deer, Cervus elaphus , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  W. Searcy Song repertoire and mate choice in birds , 1992 .

[14]  J. Mitani,et al.  Male Gibbon (Hylobates agilis) Singing Behavior: Natural History, Song Variations and Function , 2010 .

[15]  J. Raemaekers,et al.  Field playback of loud calls to gibbons (Hylobates lar): territorial, sex-specific and species-specific responses , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  J. Bradbury,et al.  The energetic cost of display in male sage grouse , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  E. H. Haimoff The organization of song in Müller’s Gibbon (Hylobates muelleri) , 1985, International Journal of Primatology.

[18]  G. Cowlishaw Song Function in Gibbons , 1992 .

[19]  J. Raemaekers,et al.  Long-Range Vocal Interactions Between Groups of Gibbons (Hylobates Lar) , 1985 .

[20]  D. Harper,et al.  The evolution of aggression: can selection generate variability? , 1988, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[21]  S. Gittins Territorial behavior in the Agile Gibbon , 1980, International Journal of Primatology.

[22]  J. Fleagle THE SIAMANG IN MALAYA , 1976 .

[23]  R. Wiley,et al.  Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: Implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations , 1978, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[24]  R. Palombit Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous ape , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  A. Whitten,et al.  The ecology of singing in Kloss gibbons (Hylobates klossii) on Siberut Island, Indonesia , 1982, International Journal of Primatology.

[26]  J. Mitani Location-specific Responses of Gibbons (Hylobates muelleri) to Male Songs , 1985 .

[27]  A. Grafen Biological signals as handicaps. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[28]  Charles H. Brown,et al.  Habitat acoustics and primate communication , 1986, American journal of primatology.

[29]  G. Parker,et al.  Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. , 1974, Journal of theoretical biology.

[30]  A. D. Johns Behavioral responses of two Malaysian primates (Hylobates lar andPresbytis melalophos) to selective logging: Vocal behavior, territoriality, and nonemigration , 1985, International Journal of Primatology.

[31]  R. H. Wiley,et al.  Reverberations and Amplitude Fluctuations in the Propagation of Sound in a Forest: Implications for Animal Communication , 1980, The American Naturalist.

[32]  T. Guilford,et al.  The corruption of honest signalling , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  M. Reid,et al.  Costliness and reliability in the singing vigour of Ipswich sparrows , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[34]  J. Raemaekers Changes through the day in the food choice of wild gibbons. , 1978, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[35]  E. H. Haimoff,et al.  Preliminary observations of wild black-crested gibbons (Hylobates concolor concolor) in Yunnan province, people's republic of China , 1987, Primates.

[36]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  THE EVOLUTION OF MONOGAMY IN LARGE PRIMATES: A NEW HYPOTHESIS AND SOME CRUCIAL TESTS , 1990 .

[37]  N. Davies,et al.  FOOD DISTRIBUTION AND A VARIABLE MATING SYSTEM IN THE DUNNOCK, PRUNELLA MODULARIS , 1984 .