No Language-Specific Activation during Linguistic Processing of Observed Actions

Background It has been suggested that cortical neural systems for language evolved from motor cortical systems, in particular from those fronto-parietal systems responding also to action observation. While previous studies have shown shared cortical systems for action – or action observation - and language, they did not address the question of whether linguistic processing of visual stimuli occurs only within a subset of fronto-parietal areas responding to action observation. If this is true, the hypothesis that language evolved from fronto-parietal systems matching action execution and action observation would be strongly reinforced. Methodology/ Principal Findings We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while subjects watched video stimuli of hand-object-interactions and control photo stimuli of the objects and performed linguistic (conceptual and phonological), and perceptual tasks. Since stimuli were identical for linguistic and perceptual tasks, differential activations had to be related to task demands. The results revealed that the linguistic tasks activated left inferior frontal areas that were subsets of a large bilateral fronto-parietal network activated during action perception. Not a single cortical area demonstrated exclusive – or even simply higher - activation for the linguistic tasks compared to the action perception task. Conclusions These results show that linguistic tasks do not only share common neural representations but essentially activate a subset of the action observation network if identical stimuli are used. Our findings strongly support the evolutionary hypothesis that fronto-parietal systems matching action execution and observation were co-opted for language, a process known as exaptation.

[1]  P. Andrews,et al.  Adaptationism – how to carry out an exaptationist program , 2002, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  C. Keysers,et al.  Empathy and the Somatotopic Auditory Mirror System in Humans , 2006, Current Biology.

[3]  A M Liberman,et al.  Perception of the speech code. , 1967, Psychological review.

[4]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Left hemisphere motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  V. Gallese Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[7]  Agnes Flöel,et al.  Language perception activates the hand motor cortex: implications for motor theories of speech perception , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[8]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Lateralization of the Human Mirror Neuron System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[12]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[13]  Jean Decety,et al.  Motivation modulates the activity of the human mirror-neuron system. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2004, Neuron.

[15]  R. Töpper,et al.  Motor cortex hand area and speech: implications for the development of language , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  R. Sparing,et al.  Task‐dependent modulation of functional connectivity between hand motor cortices and neuronal networks underlying language and music: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study in humans , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  The human action recognition system and its relationship to Broca’s area: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[19]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Congruent Embodied Representations for Visually Presented Actions and Linguistic Phrases Describing Actions , 2006, Current Biology.

[20]  M. Arbib From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  Bernard Mazoyer,et al.  Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[23]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  The essential role of Broca's area in imitation , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[25]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Speech‐induced changes in corticospinal excitability , 1996, Annals of neurology.

[26]  A. Liberman,et al.  On the relation of speech to language , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  M. Corballis From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness , 2003, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.