Enhancement of cognitive control by approach and avoidance motivational states

Affective variables have been shown to impact working memory and cognitive control. Theoretical arguments suggest that the functional impact of emotion on cognition might be mediated through shifting action dispositions related to changes in motivational orientation. The current study examined the effects of positive and negative affect on performance via direct manipulation of motivational state in tasks with high demands on cognitive control. Experiment 1 examined the effects of monetary reward on task-switching performance, while Experiment 2 examined the effects of both rewards and punishments on working memory, using primary (liquid) reinforcers. In both experiments, dissociable trial-by-trial and contextual (block-related) enhancements of cognitive control during task performance were observed in relationship to motivational incentive value. Performance enhancements were equivalent in the reward and punishment conditions, but were differentially impacted by individual difference measures of trait reward and punishment sensitivity. Together, the results suggest both common and specific mechanisms by which approach and avoidance motivational states influence cognitive control, via activation of reward and punishment processing systems.

[1]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Primary and Secondary Rewards Differentially Modulate Neural Activity Dynamics during Working Memory , 2010, PloS one.

[2]  Hannah S. Locke,et al.  Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  Body Locomotion as Regulatory Process , 2009, Psychological science.

[4]  L. Pessoa How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  R. Holland,et al.  Regulating cognitive control through approach-avoidance motor actions , 2008, Cognition.

[6]  T. Braver,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: Differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Avram J. Holmes,et al.  Dissociable recruitment of rostral anterior cingulate and inferior frontal cortex in emotional response inhibition , 2008, NeuroImage.

[8]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Approach-Motivated Positive Affect Reduces Breadth of Attention , 2008, Psychological science.

[9]  Sara M. Levens,et al.  Emotion processing effects on interference resolution in working memory. , 2008, Emotion.

[10]  Hannah S. Locke,et al.  Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  L. Pessoa On the relationship between emotion and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Effects of incentive on working memory capacity: behavioral and pupillometric data. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[13]  Hannah S. Locke Motivational and personality influences on cognitive control and task performance , 2008 .

[14]  K. Lesch,et al.  Dopamine and cognitive control: the prospect of monetary gains influences the balance between flexibility and stability in a set‐shifting paradigm , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[15]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Motivation sharpens exogenous spatial attention. , 2007, Emotion.

[16]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  The English Lexicon Project , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[17]  Sonia J. Bishop,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. , 2007, Emotion.

[19]  Daniel C. Krawczyk,et al.  Reward modulation of prefrontal and visual association cortex during an incentive working memory task , 2007, Brain Research.

[20]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[21]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm , 2007 .

[22]  A. Anderson,et al.  Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[24]  R. O’Reilly Biologically Based Computational Models of High-Level Cognition , 2006, Science.

[25]  Grant C. Baldwin,et al.  A test of the regulatory fit hypothesis in perceptual classification learning , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[26]  G. Dreisbach How positive affect modulates cognitive control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance capability , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Monetary incentives enhance processing in brain regions mediating top-down control of attention. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Arthur B Markman,et al.  The Interaction of Payoff Structure and Regulatory Focus in Classification , 2005, Psychological science.

[30]  T. Braver,et al.  Affective personality differences in neural processing efficiency confirmed using fMRI , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[31]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  Working Memory Capacity , 2005 .

[33]  Julie A Fiez,et al.  Integrating rewards and cognition in the frontal cortex , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[34]  M. Roesch,et al.  Neuronal Activity Related to Reward Value and Motivation in Primate Frontal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[35]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  A functional neuroimaging study of motivation and executive function , 2004, NeuroImage.

[37]  S. Corkin,et al.  Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. , 2003, Emotion.

[38]  Jeremy R. Gray,et al.  12. Integration of emotion and cognitive control: A neurocomputational hypothesis of dynamic goal regulation , 2002 .

[39]  J B Poline,et al.  The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in humans: An fMRI study , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  M. Raichle,et al.  Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  R. Engle Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention , 2002 .

[43]  Karen Gasper,et al.  Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information , 2002, Psychological science.

[44]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[45]  J. Gray,et al.  Emotional modulation of cognitive control: approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[46]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[47]  E. Rolls,et al.  Representation of pleasant and aversive taste in the human brain. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[49]  E. Higgins,et al.  Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention pride , 2001 .

[50]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[51]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[52]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Introversion and working memory: central executive differences , 2000 .

[53]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[54]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching , 2000 .

[55]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes , 2000 .

[56]  John M. Zelenski,et al.  Susceptibility to affect: a comparison of three personality taxonomies. , 1999, Journal of personality.

[57]  J. Jonides,et al.  Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.

[58]  E. Tory Higgins,et al.  Promotion and prevention as a motivational duality: Implications for evaluative processes. , 1999 .

[59]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[60]  Calmet Meteorological Model A User's Guide for the , 1999 .

[61]  Andrew J. Tomarkenand,et al.  Frontal Brain Asymmetry and Depression: A Self-regulatory Perspective , 1998 .

[62]  E. Higgins,et al.  Regulatory Focus and Strategic Inclinations : Promotion and Prevention in Decision-Making , 1997 .

[63]  Wendy Heller,et al.  regional Brain Activity in Emotion: A Framework for Understanding Cognition in Depresion , 1997 .

[64]  E. Higgins,et al.  Beyond pleasure and pain. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[65]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[66]  Curt Burgess,et al.  Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence , 1996 .

[67]  M. Oaksford,et al.  Mood, reasoning, and central executive processes. , 1996 .

[68]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[69]  R. Davidson Cerebral asymmetry, emotion, and affective style. , 1995 .

[70]  B. Parkinson,et al.  Emotion and motivation , 1995 .

[71]  C. Carver,et al.  Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .

[72]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[73]  A. Isen,et al.  Positive affect and decision making. , 1993 .

[74]  R. Larsen,et al.  Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[75]  P. Ekman,et al.  Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and brain physiology. I. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[76]  C. Carver,et al.  Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control-Process View. , 1990 .

[77]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Sensation seeking, Eysenck's personality dimensions and reinforcement sensitivity in concept formation☆ , 1990 .

[78]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[79]  M. Humphreys,et al.  Personality, motivation, and performance: a theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing. , 1984, Psychological review.

[80]  J. Gray,et al.  A Critique of Eysenck’s Theory of Personality , 1981 .

[81]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[82]  H. Eysenck Biological Basis of Personality , 1963, Nature.

[83]  F. Plum Handbook of Physiology. , 1960 .

[84]  J. Easterbrook The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. , 1959, Psychological review.

[85]  J. Stainer,et al.  The Emotions , 1882, Nature.

[86]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[87]  M. Manosevitz,et al.  High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , 2022 .