Distinguishing spontaneous from deliberate mind wandering in adolescents: The role of attentional control and depressive symptoms.

Research on individual differences in mind wandering (MW) in healthy adults has supported the separation of spontaneous and deliberate MW given their different association with a number of psychological traits. This study aimed to further test this distinction by investigating the associations of the two types of MW with attentional control and depressive symptomatology in adolescents. Data were collected on 439 9-10th graders, who completed measures of spontaneous and deliberate MW, distractibility, difficulty in attentional shifting, and depressive symptomatology. A multivariate three-level multiple regression revealed that distractibility was a significant predictor of only spontaneous MW, whereas difficulties in attentional shifting and depressive symptoms were significant predictors of both types of MW. However, the last two variables were stronger predictors for spontaneous than for deliberate MW. These findings provide support for the distinction, beyond their theoretical and empirical overlap, between the two types of MW also in adolescents and suggest the independent contribution of cognitive functioning and depressive symptoms. Statement of contribution What is already known? Studies on mind wandering (MW) in adults have shown that spontaneous and deliberate MW reflect dissociable cognitive experiences. In adults, trait-level tendency to spontaneous MW seems to reflect difficulties in controlled attentional processing. The majority of studies on MW in adolescents have treated this phenomenon as unitary and found a relation with negative affect, stress, and self-esteem. What does this study add? This is the first study to examine cognitive functioning and depressive symptoms as correlates of spontaneous and deliberate MW in adolescents. We found that distractibility is a significant predictor of only spontaneous MW. Difficulties in attentional shifting and depressive symptoms are significant predictors of both types of MW, although stronger for spontaneous MW. The findings support the distinction, beyond their theoretical and empirical overlap, between the two types of MW also in adolescents and call for future studies on the function of deliberate and spontaneous MW across development.

[1]  David Marcusson-Clavertz,et al.  Psychometric Properties of the Spontaneous and Deliberate Mind Wandering Scales , 2019, European Journal of Psychological Assessment.

[2]  C. Chiorri,et al.  Replicability of the Psychometric Properties of Trait-Levels Measures of Spontaneous and Deliberate Mind Wandering , 2019, European Journal of Psychological Assessment.

[3]  D. Smilek,et al.  Depression, anxiety, and stress and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. , 2019, Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice.

[4]  T. Stafford,et al.  Cognitive control across adolescence: Dynamic adjustments and mind-wandering. , 2020, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  A. Moustafa,et al.  Off‐track thoughts: Intentional and unintentional mind wandering in Alzheimer's disease , 2019, Geriatrics & gerontology international.

[6]  E. Somer,et al.  Trapped in a Daydream: Daily Elevations in Maladaptive Daydreaming Are Associated With Daily Psychopathological Symptoms , 2018, Front. Psychiatry.

[7]  C. Chiorri,et al.  Individual differences in self-consciousness and mind wandering: Further evidence for a dissociation between spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering , 2018 .

[8]  G. Corazza,et al.  Exploring the Link Between Mind Wandering, Mindfulness, and Creativity: A Multidimensional Approach , 2018 .

[9]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Mind-Wandering With and Without Intention , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  G. Oettingen,et al.  Pleasure Now, Pain Later , 2016, Psychological science.

[11]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[12]  S. Majerus,et al.  Relationships between mind-wandering and attentional control abilities in young adults and adolescents. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Peter Totterdell,et al.  Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[14]  Jonathan W. Schooler,et al.  Young and restless: validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[15]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. , 2013, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[16]  E. Menesini,et al.  Codevelopment of Delinquent and Depressive Symptoms Across Adolescence: Time-Invariant and Time-Varying Effects of School and Social Failure , 2012, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[17]  Nicola C. Anderson,et al.  Everyday Attention: Variation in Mind Wandering and Memory in a Lecture , 2012 .

[18]  M. Bartels,et al.  A systematic review of prospective studies on attention problems and academic achievement , 2010, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[19]  J. Smallwood,et al.  Mind-wandering and dysphoria , 2007 .

[20]  A. MacLeod,et al.  Associations between phenomenal and objective aspects of concentration problems in depressed patients. , 1988, British journal of psychology.