Evidence of rapid correlation-based perceptual category learning by 4-month-olds

Young infants are very sensitive to feature distribution information in the environment. However, existing work suggests that they do not make use of correlation information to form certain perceptual categories until at least 7 months of age. We suggest that the failure to use correlation information is a by-product of familiarization procedures that encourage infants to over encode individual exemplars rather than relations across exemplars. By changing the exemplar presentation regime to one in which exemplars are rapidly (2s durations) and repeatedly presented we find that 4-month-olds can form perceptual categories on the basis of feature correlation information. In addition, this ability emerges rapidly between 114 and 134 days. We argue that the ability to process correlation information is present very early on but that the demonstration of that ability in categorization tasks is mediated by the demands of the task the infant is tested with. Copyright (c) 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  B. Roder,et al.  Infants' Preferences for Familiarity and Novelty During the Course of Visual Processing. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[2]  David J. Field,et al.  Wavelets, vision and the statistics of natural scenes , 1999, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.

[3]  D. Mareschal,et al.  From Parts to Wholes: Mechanisms of Development in Infant Visual Object Processing. , 2004, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[4]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[5]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  M. Strauss,et al.  Abstraction of prototypical information by adults and 10-month-old infants. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[7]  D. Kleinbaum,et al.  Applied Regression Analysis and Other Multivariate Methods , 1978 .

[8]  Denis Mareschal,et al.  Mechanisms of Categorization in Infancy. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[9]  S. Luck,et al.  The development of visual short-term memory capacity in infants. , 2003, Child development.

[10]  Dianne F Catherwood,et al.  Assessing the primary moments in infant encoding of compound visual stimuli , 1996 .

[11]  Robert M. French,et al.  Asymmetric interference in 3- to 4-month-olds' sequential category learning , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  Martial Mermillod,et al.  The role of bottom-up processing in perceptual categorization by 3- to 4-month-old infants: simulations and data. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  Denis Mareschal,et al.  Models of habituation in infancy , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Barbara A. Younger,et al.  Developmental change in infants' perception of correlations among attributes. , 1986, Child development.

[15]  B. Younger,et al.  Infants' detection of correlations among feature categories. , 1990, Child development.

[16]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  The categorical representation of visual pattern information by young infants , 1987, Cognition.

[17]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  ARTICLE WITH PEER COMMENTARIES AND RESPONSE Learning to perceive object unity: a connectionist account , 2002 .

[18]  R E Lasky,et al.  The processing of tachistoscopically presented visual stimuli by five-month-old infants. , 1980, Child development.

[19]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[20]  R. French,et al.  A connectionist account of asymmetric category learning in early infancy. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[21]  David J. Field,et al.  What Is the Goal of Sensory Coding? , 1994, Neural Computation.

[22]  L. Cohen,et al.  How infants process addition and subtraction events , 2002 .

[23]  Paul C Quinn,et al.  Neural Markers of Categorization in 6-Month-Old Infants , 2006, Psychological science.

[24]  B. Younger,et al.  Development of Categorization Skills: Changes in the Nature or Structure of Infant Form Categories?. , 1988 .

[25]  P. Quinn,et al.  Categorization in infancy , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  The nature and structure of infant form categories , 1983 .

[27]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[28]  R. French Catastrophic forgetting in connectionist networks , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Pattern recognition and categorization , 1972 .

[30]  M. Posner,et al.  Retention of Abstract Ideas. , 1970 .

[31]  Dianne F Catherwood,et al.  Exploring the seminal phase in infant memory for color and shape , 1994 .

[32]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Pre-stereoptic binocular vision in infants , 1986, Vision Research.

[33]  B. Younger The segregation of items into categories by ten-month-old infants. , 1985, Child development.

[34]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Induction of category distributions: a framework for classification learning. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  E. Newport,et al.  Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .

[36]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.