Unpredictability and complexity of print-to-speech correspondences increase reliance on lexical processes: more evidence for the orthographic depth hypothesis
暂无分享,去创建一个
Xenia Schmalz | Eva Marinus | Elisabeth Beyersmann | E. Marinus | Eddy Cavalli | Xenia Schmalz | Elisabeth Beyersmann | Eddy Cavalli
[1] Bernard Lété,et al. MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[2] V. Mann,et al. Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children , 2002 .
[3] Michael T. Turvey,et al. The Serbo-Croatian Orthography Constrains the Reader to a Phonologically Analytic Strategy , 2017 .
[4] Leif D. Nelson,et al. False-Positive Psychology , 2011, Psychological science.
[5] H. Wimmer,et al. The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison , 1997, Cognition.
[6] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. The preparation of syllables in speech production , 2004 .
[7] D. Bates,et al. Parsimonious Mixed Models , 2015, 1506.04967.
[8] Rebecca Treiman,et al. The English Lexicon Project , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[9] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] Kenneth I Forster,et al. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[11] Ram Frostt,et al. Prelexical and Postlexical Strategies in Reading : Evidence from a Deep and a Shallow Orthography * , 2009 .
[12] Michael J Cortese,et al. Regularity effects in word naming: What are they? , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[13] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[14] L. Katz,et al. The reading process is different for different orthographies : the orthographic depth hypothesis , 1992 .
[15] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Hidden multiplicity in exploratory multiway ANOVA: Prevalence and remedies , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[16] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Age of Acquisition Effects in Word Reading and Other Tasks , 2002 .
[17] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Subtlex-UK: A New and Improved Word Frequency Database for British English , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[18] Arnaud Rey,et al. A phoneme effect in visual word recognition , 1998, Cognition.
[19] Gordon D. A. Brown,et al. Contextual Diversity, Not Word Frequency, Determines Word-Naming and Lexical Decision Times , 2006, Psychological science.
[20] Marcus Taft,et al. Reading and the Mental Lexicon , 1991 .
[21] S. Andrews. Phonological recoding: Is the regularity effect consistent? , 1982 .
[22] Conrad Perry,et al. The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German , 2000 .
[23] M. Brysbaert,et al. The use of film subtitles to estimate word frequencies , 2007, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[24] Ram Frost,et al. Towards a universal model of reading , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[25] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Semantic effects in single-word naming. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[26] Dan Chateau,et al. Spelling–sound consistency effects in disyllabic word naming , 2003 .
[27] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[28] L. Katz,et al. Relation between pronunciation and recognition of printed words in deep and shallow orthographies. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] Marco Zorzi,et al. Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.
[30] A. Jacobs,et al. The word frequency effect: a review of recent developments and implications for the choice of frequency estimates in German. , 2011, Experimental psychology.
[31] Xenia Schmalz,et al. Quantifying the reliance on different sublexical correspondences in German and English , 2014 .
[32] Rebecca Treiman,et al. Influence of consonantal context on the pronunciation of vowels: A comparison of human readers and computational models , 2003, Cognition.
[33] Elisabeth Dévière,et al. Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R , 2009 .
[34] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. Is the dual-route theory possible in phonetically regular languages? , 1987, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[35] Marc Brysbaert,et al. The French Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords , 2010, Behavior research methods.
[36] D. Jared. Spelling-Sound Consistency and Regularity Effects in Word Naming , 2002 .
[37] Walter Daelemans,et al. Measuring the Complexity of Writing Systems , 1994, J. Quant. Linguistics.
[38] Serje Robidoux,et al. Hierarchical clustering analysis of reading aloud data: a new technique for evaluating the performance of computational models , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[39] Uta Frith,et al. Differences in Phonological Recoding in German- and English-Speaking Children. , 1998 .
[40] Eva Marinus,et al. Density and length in the neighborhood: Explaining cross-linguistic differences in learning to read in English and Dutch. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[41] Xenia Schmalz,et al. Getting to the bottom of orthographic depth , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[42] M Coltheart,et al. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.
[43] Sallyanne Palethorpe,et al. Nonword reading: comparing dual-route cascaded and connectionist dual-process models with human data. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] Eva Marinus,et al. Variability in the word-reading performance of dyslexic readers: Effects of letter length, phoneme length and digraph presence , 2010, Cortex.
[45] H. Wimmer,et al. Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies , 2003, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[46] D. Balota,et al. Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[47] Debra Jared,et al. Spelling-Sound Consistency Affects the Naming of High-Frequency Words , 1997 .
[48] Kristina Moll,et al. Effects of orthographic consistency on eye movement behavior: German and English children and adults process the same words differently. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[49] M. Coltheart,et al. Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on nonword reading , 1998 .
[50] R. Baayen,et al. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .
[51] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. Effects of syllable preparation and syllable frequency in speech production: Further evidence for syllabic units at a post-lexical level , 2009 .
[52] Laura M. Steacy,et al. Modeling Polymorphemic Word Recognition , 2016, Journal of learning disabilities.
[53] Marc Brysbaert,et al. The British Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.
[54] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. The basis of consistency effects in word naming , 1990 .
[55] Conrad Perry,et al. Speed of lexical and nonlexical processing in French: The case of the regularity effect , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[56] Athanassios Protopapas,et al. Check Vocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[57] Marco Zorzi,et al. Beyond single syllables: Large-scale modeling of reading aloud with the Connectionist Dual Process (CDP++) model , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[58] M. Brysbaert,et al. Practice Effects in Large-Scale Visual Word Recognition Studies: A Lexical Decision Study on 14,000 Dutch Mono- and Disyllabic Words and Nonwords , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[59] P. H. Seymour,et al. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. , 2003, British journal of psychology.
[60] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[61] L. Katz,et al. Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: a multilingual comparison. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] R. Glushko. The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .
[63] D. Barr,et al. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.
[64] D. Balota,et al. Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words , 2009 .
[65] Sally Andrews,et al. Rule and analogy mechanisms in reading nonwords: Hough dou peapel rede gnew wirds? , 1998 .
[66] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[67] D. Share. On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: the perils of overreliance on an "outlier" orthography. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.