Goal-directed aiming under restricted viewing conditions with confirmatory sensory feedback.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. Fitts,et al. INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[2] Matthew Heath,et al. Visuomotor Memory for Target Location in Near and Far Reaching Spaces , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.
[3] Matthew Heath,et al. Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. , 2003, Motor control.
[4] Richard D. Morey,et al. Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .
[5] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[6] M. Shiffrar,et al. Fitts's Law Holds for Action Perception , 2007, Psychological science.
[7] Denis Cousineau,et al. Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .
[8] S. Glover,et al. Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[9] W. Helsen,et al. A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[10] Denis Mottet,et al. Informational constraints in human precision aiming , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.
[11] R. Gray,et al. Multisensory Information in the Control of Complex Motor Actions , 2008 .
[12] Kristen L. Macuga,et al. Differential contributions of the superior and inferior parietal cortex to feedback versus feedforward control of tools , 2014, NeuroImage.
[13] Brendan D. Cameron,et al. Target modality affects visually guided online control of reaching , 2015, Vision Research.
[14] Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al. Measuring Neuromuscular Control Dynamics During Car Following With Continuous Haptic Feedback , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, Part B (Cybernetics).
[15] I. Scott MacKenzie,et al. Fitts' Law as a Research and Design Tool in Human-Computer Interaction , 1992, Hum. Comput. Interact..
[16] R Plamondon,et al. Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[17] Y Agid,et al. Temporal limits of spatial working memory in humans , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.
[18] L. Tremblay,et al. Effect of task-specific execution on accuracy of imagined aiming movements , 2015, Neuroscience Letters.
[19] Shumin Zhai,et al. What You Feel Must Be What You See: Adding Tactile Feedback to the Trackpoint , 1999, INTERACT.
[20] Andy Cockburn,et al. Multimodal feedback for the acquisition of small targets , 2005, Ergonomics.
[21] Annie W. Y. Ng,et al. Finger response times to visual, auditory and tactile modality stimuli , 2012, IMECS 2012.
[22] Philip N. Sabes,et al. Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] D. Elliott. Chapter 2 Intermittent Versus Continuous Control of Manual Aiming Movements , 1992 .
[24] S. Lederman,et al. Texture perception: studies of intersensory organization using a discrepancy paradigm, and visual versus tactual psychophysics. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] M Akamatsu,et al. Please Scroll down for Article Ergonomics a Comparison of Tactile, Auditory, and Visual Feedback in a Pointing Task Using a Mouse-type Device , 2022 .
[26] D. H. Warren,et al. Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.
[27] Robert Gray,et al. A Comparison of Tactile, Visual, and Auditory Warnings for Rear-End Collision Prevention in Simulated Driving , 2008, Hum. Factors.
[28] D. Knill,et al. The role of memory in visually guided reaching. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[29] R A Abrams,et al. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.
[30] D. Wolpert,et al. When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.
[31] Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al. THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .
[32] Stephen H Scott,et al. Dynamic Multisensory Integration: Somatosensory Speed Trumps Visual Accuracy during Feedback Control , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[33] G. DeAngelis,et al. Multisensory integration: psychophysics, neurophysiology, and computation , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[34] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[35] Jiajia Yang,et al. Fitts' law holds for pointing movements under conditions of restricted visual feedback. , 2010, Human movement science.
[36] T. O. Kvålseth. An alternative to Fitts’ law , 1980 .
[37] William R. Wyatt,et al. Effects of Auditory Feedback on Movement Time in a Fitts Task , 2010, Journal of motor behavior.
[38] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[39] M S Graziano,et al. Coding the locations of objects in the dark. , 1997, Science.
[40] Jiajia Yang,et al. Effects of aging on pointing movements under restricted visual feedback conditions. , 2015, Human movement science.
[41] J. Duysens,et al. The breakdown of Fitts’ law in rapid, reciprocal aiming movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] H S Vitense,et al. Multimodal feedback: an assessment of performance and mental workload , 2003, Ergonomics.