Exploring the Fundamental Dynamics of Error-Based Motor Learning Using a Stationary Predictive-Saccade Task

The maintenance of movement accuracy uses prior performance errors to correct future motor plans; this motor-learning process ensures that movements remain quick and accurate. The control of predictive saccades, in which anticipatory movements are made to future targets before visual stimulus information becomes available, serves as an ideal paradigm to analyze how the motor system utilizes prior errors to drive movements to a desired goal. Predictive saccades constitute a stationary process (the mean and to a rough approximation the variability of the data do not vary over time, unlike a typical motor adaptation paradigm). This enables us to study inter-trial correlations, both on a trial-by-trial basis and across long blocks of trials. Saccade errors are found to be corrected on a trial-by-trial basis in a direction-specific manner (the next saccade made in the same direction will reflect a correction for errors made on the current saccade). Additionally, there is evidence for a second, modulating process that exhibits long memory. That is, performance information, as measured via inter-trial correlations, is strongly retained across a large number of saccades (about 100 trials). Together, this evidence indicates that the dynamics of motor learning exhibit complexities that must be carefully considered, as they cannot be fully described with current state-space (ARMA) modeling efforts.

[1]  Clayton E Curtis,et al.  Obligatory adaptation of saccade gains. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Marius Ooms,et al.  Inference and Forecasting for Fractional Autoregressive Integrated Moving Average Models, with an application to US and UK inflation , 1999 .

[3]  F R Robinson,et al.  Visual error is the stimulus for saccade gain adaptation. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[4]  Joseph H. Goldberg,et al.  Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols , 2000, ETRA.

[5]  Bruce J. West,et al.  Fractal physiology , 1994, IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine.

[6]  M. Cynader,et al.  Adaptability of the vestibulo-ocular reflex to vision reversal in strobe reared cats , 1981, Brain Research.

[7]  Clive W. J. Granger,et al.  An introduction to long-memory time series models and fractional differencing , 2001 .

[8]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Predictive Control of Eye Tracking Movements , 1962 .

[9]  L. E. Ross,et al.  Children's and adults' predictive saccades to square-wave targets , 1987, Vision Research.

[10]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sequences of Predictive Saccades Are Correlated Over a Span of ∼2 s and Produce a Fractal Time Series , 2005 .

[11]  John G. Proakis,et al.  Probability, random variables and stochastic processes , 1985, IEEE Trans. Acoust. Speech Signal Process..

[12]  M. Petrides,et al.  The effect of spatial and temporal information on saccades and neural activity in oculomotor structures. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  G. Jones,et al.  Extreme vestibulo‐ocular adaptation induced by prolonged optical reversal of vision , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  M. Treisman,et al.  American Psychological Association, Inc, A Theory of Criterion Setting With an Application to Sequential Dependencies , 2022 .

[15]  Steven B. Lowen,et al.  Fractal-Based Point Processes , 2005 .

[16]  A. Bastian Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  D. Zambarbieri,et al.  SACCADIC EYE MOVEMENTS TO PREDICTABLE VISUAL AND AUDITORY TARGETS , 1987 .

[18]  G. Jones,et al.  An adaptive neural model compatible with plastic changes induced in the human vestibulo-ocular reflex by prolonged optical reversal of vision , 1976, Brain Research.

[19]  Jan Beran,et al.  Statistics for long-memory processes , 1994 .

[20]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[21]  S Roberts,et al.  Analysis of correlations and search for evidence of deterministic chaos in rhythmic motor control by the human brain. , 2000, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[22]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Theories and models for 1/f(beta) noise in human movement science. , 2009, Human movement science.

[23]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in rhesus macaques. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  M. Riley,et al.  IN FRACTAL PHYSIOLOGY , 2022 .

[25]  R. Ivry,et al.  The Predictive Brain State: Timing Deficiency in Traumatic Brain Injury? , 2008, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[26]  Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al.  Motor Adaptation Scaled by the Difficulty of a Secondary Cognitive Task , 2008, PloS one.

[27]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Multiscaled randomness: A possible source of 1/f noise in biology. , 1996, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[28]  R. Leipus,et al.  Testing for long memory in the presence of a general trend , 2001, Journal of Applied Probability.

[29]  Patrick J Drew,et al.  Models and properties of power-law adaptation in neural systems. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  W. Becker,et al.  Accuracy of saccadic eye movements and maintenance of eccentric eye positions in the dark. , 1973, Vision research.

[31]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sensory, motor, and combined contexts for context-specific adaptation of saccade gain in humans , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[32]  T Anstis,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Andrew S. French,et al.  The Power Law of Sensory Adaptation: Simulation by a Model of Excitability in Spider Mechanoreceptor Neurons , 2007, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[34]  Mark J Shelhamer,et al.  Nonlinear Dynamics in Physiology: A State-space Approach , 2006 .

[35]  H. Deubel,et al.  Cerebellar lesions impair rapid saccade amplitude adaptation , 2001, Neurology.

[36]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical Systems , 2006, Neural Computation.

[37]  Jeffrey D. Scargle,et al.  Fractal-Based Point Processes , 2007, Technometrics.

[38]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[39]  Benoit B. Mandelbrot,et al.  Fractal Geometry of Nature , 1984 .

[40]  R C Miall,et al.  The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[41]  W. Zangemeister,et al.  Disturbance of Predictive Response Initiation of Eye and Head Movements in Cerebellar Patients , 2008, European Neurology.

[42]  P. Young,et al.  Time series analysis, forecasting and control , 1972, IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control.

[43]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Fractal mechanisms and heart rate dynamics. Long-range correlations and their breakdown with disease. , 1995, Journal of electrocardiology.

[44]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Sensory Processing of Motor Inaccuracy Depends on Previously Performed Movement and on Subsequent Motor Corrections: A Study of the Saccadic System , 2011, PloS one.

[45]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Human cognition and a pile of sand: a discussion on serial correlations and self-organized criticality. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[46]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimation and interpretation of 1/fα noise in human cognition , 2004 .

[47]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Spontaneous recovery of motor memory during saccade adaptation. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Simon Farrell,et al.  Estimation and interpretation of 1/falpha noise in human cognition. , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[49]  Naama Brenner,et al.  History-Dependent Multiple-Time-Scale Dynamics in a Single-Neuron Model , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  Marius Ooms,et al.  Computational aspects of maximum likelihood estimation of autoregressive fractionally integrated moving average models , 2003, Comput. Stat. Data Anal..

[51]  Rangarajan,et al.  Integrated approach to the assessment of long range correlation in time series data , 2000, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[52]  Scott E. Bevans,et al.  Effect of visual error size on saccade adaptation in monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  Yanqing Chen,et al.  Long Memory Processes ( 1 / f α Type) in Human Coordination , 1997 .

[54]  P. Thier,et al.  Reduced saccadic resilience and impaired saccadic adaptation due to cerebellar disease , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[55]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Multiple timescales and uncertainty in motor adaptation , 2006, NIPS.

[57]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[58]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[59]  J. Yelnik,et al.  Involvement of the cerebellar thalamus in human saccade adaptation , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[60]  N. Shimizu [Neurology of eye movements]. , 2000, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[61]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Adaptive Control of Saccades in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Explaining savings for visuomotor adaptation: linear time-invariant state-space models are not sufficient. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  Albert F Fuchs,et al.  Activity changes in monkey superior colliculus during saccade adaptation. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  Pierre Morel,et al.  Optimal and Suboptimal Use of Postsaccadic Vision in Sequences of Saccades , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Saccadic gain modification: visual error drives motor adaptation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Cerebellar Influence in Oculomotor Phase‐Transition Behavior , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[68]  P. Devynck,et al.  The Hurst exponent and long-time correlation , 2000 .

[69]  J. Vercher,et al.  Mechanisms of short-term saccadic adaptation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Saccades exhibit abrupt transition between reactive and predictive; predictive saccade sequences have long-term correlations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.