Cortical Representations Sensitive to the Number of Perceived Auditory Objects Emerge between 2 and 4 Months of Age: Electrophysiological Evidence

Sound waves emitted by two or more simultaneous sources reach the ear as one complex waveform. Auditory scene analysis involves parsing a complex waveform into separate perceptual representations of the sound sources [Bregman, A. S. Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sounds. London: MIT Press, 1990]. Harmonicity provides an important cue for auditory scene analysis. Normally, harmonics at integer multiples of a fundamental frequency are perceived as one sound with a pitch corresponding to the fundamental frequency. However, when one harmonic in such a complex, pitch-evoking sound is sufficiently mistuned, that harmonic emerges from the complex tone and is perceived as a separate auditory object. Previous work has shown that the percept of two objects is indexed in both children and adults by the object-related negativity component of the ERP derived from EEG recordings [Alain, C., Arnott, S. T., & Picton, T. W. Bottom–up and top–down influences on auditory scene analysis: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1072–1089, 2001]. Here we examine the emergence of object-related responses to an 8% harmonic mistuning in infants between 2 and 12 months of age. Two-month-old infants showed no significant object-related response. However, in 4- to 12-month-old infants, a significant frontally positive component was present, and by 8–12 months, a significant frontocentral object-related negativity was present, similar to that seen in older children and adults. This is in accordance with previous research demonstrating that infants younger than 4 months of age do not integrate harmonic information to perceive pitch when the fundamental is missing [He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J. Maturation of cortical mismatch mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change. Neuropsychologia, 47, 218–229, 2009]. The results indicate that the ability to use harmonic information to segregate simultaneous sounds emerges at the cortical level between 2 and 4 months of age.

[1]  B. Moore,et al.  Thresholds for hearing mistuned partials as separate tones in harmonic complexes. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  M. Hyde Newborn hearing screening programs: overview. , 2005, The Journal of otolaryngology.

[3]  C Alain,et al.  Age-related changes in detecting a mistuned harmonic. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  E. Lopez-Poveda,et al.  The neurophysiological bases of auditory perception , 2010 .

[5]  S McAdams,et al.  Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  Takako Fujioka,et al.  Neural Representation of Transposed Melody in Infants at 6 Months of Age , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[7]  W M Hartmann,et al.  The pitch of a mistuned harmonic: evidence for a template model. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Claude Alain,et al.  Age-Related Differences in Neuromagnetic Brain Activity Underlying Concurrent Sound Perception , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Terence W. Picton,et al.  Effects of Attention on Neuroelectric Correlates of Auditory Stream Segregation , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Ian C. Bruce,et al.  Effects of Peripheral Tuning on the Auditory Nerve’s Representation of Speech Envelope and Temporal Fine Structure Cues , 2010 .

[11]  L. Trainor,et al.  Musical experience, plasticity, and maturation: issues in measuring developmental change using EEG and MEG , 2012, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[12]  L. Trainor,et al.  Processing simultaneous auditory objects: infants' ability to detect mistuning in harmonic complexes. , 2012, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  I. Winkler,et al.  Auditory stream segregation processes operate similarly in school-aged children and adults , 2001, Hearing Research.

[14]  J. Jensen,et al.  Development of Basic Auditory Discrimination in Preschool Children , 1993 .

[15]  P. C. Pandey,et al.  The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America , 1939 .

[16]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Developmental changes in distinguishing concurrent auditory objects. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Chao He,et al.  Mismatch Responses to Pitch Changes in Early Infancy , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  L. Demany Auditory stream segregation in infancy , 1982 .

[20]  Albert S. Bregman,et al.  The Auditory Scene. (Book Reviews: Auditory Scene Analysis. The Perceptual Organization of Sound.) , 1990 .

[21]  J. L. Goldstein,et al.  Mechanisms of signal analysis and pattern perception in periodicity pitch. , 1978, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[22]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Comparison of artifact correction methods for infant EEG applied to extraction of event-related potential signals , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  Gavin M Bidelman,et al.  Experience-dependent plasticity in pitch encoding: from brainstem to auditory cortex , 2012, Neuroreport.

[24]  L. Trainor,et al.  Finding the Pitch of the Missing Fundamental in Infants , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  L. Trainor Developmental Psychophysiology: Event-Related Potential (ERP) Measures in Auditory Development Research , 2007 .

[26]  W M Hartmann,et al.  Pitch, periodicity, and auditory organization. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  T. Sukumaran Newborn hearing screening program , 2011, Indian Pediatrics.

[28]  L. W. Olsho,et al.  Auditory Frequency Discrimination in Infancy , 1982 .

[29]  J L Cranford,et al.  Brief-tone frequency discrimination by children. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[30]  S McAdams,et al.  Organization and discrimination of repeating sound sequences by newborn infants. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Claude Alain,et al.  Effects of Attentional Load on Auditory Scene Analysis , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  James P. Reilly,et al.  A Simple and Fast Algorithm for Automatic Suppression of High-Amplitude Artifacts in EEG Data , 2007, 2007 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing - ICASSP '07.

[33]  T. Picton,et al.  Bottom-up and top-down influences on auditory scene analysis: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.