Masked form priming with extreme transposition
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. D. Lima,et al. Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: effects of word-initial letter sequence. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Sharon M. Thomas,et al. Assessing the importance of letter pairs in initial, exterior, and interior positions in reading. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] Albrecht W. Inhoff,et al. Integrating information across fixations during reading: The use of orthographic bodies and of exterior letters. , 1995 .
[4] Sharon M. Thomas,et al. Assessing the importance of letter pairs in reading-parafoveal processing is not the only view: reply to Inhoff, Radach, Eiter, and Skelly (2003). , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[5] C. Whitney. How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] Timothy R. Jordan,et al. Perceiving exterior letters of words: differential influences of letter-fragment and non-letter-fragment masks. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .
[8] T. M. Ellison,et al. Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.
[9] J Grainger,et al. The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[10] Christine Guerrera. Flexibility and constraint in lexical access: Explorations in transposed-letter priming , 2004 .
[11] R S Berndt,et al. A new model of letter string encoding: simulating right neglect dyslexia. , 1999, Progress in brain research.
[12] S. Lupker,et al. Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .
[13] Kenneth I Forster,et al. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[14] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .
[15] Max Coltheart,et al. Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .
[16] M. Coltheart,et al. 358,534 nonwords: The ARC Nonword Database , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[17] S. Lupker,et al. Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[18] Michael C. Mozer,et al. Perception of multiple objects - a connectionist approach , 1991, Neural network modeling and connectionism.
[19] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception , 2004 .
[20] J. Grainger,et al. Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .
[21] Philip T Quinlan,et al. Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.
[22] Jeffrey S Bowers,et al. Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] K. Forster,et al. REPETITION PRIMING AND FREQUENCY ATTENUATION IN LEXICAL ACCESS , 1984 .
[24] H. Kucera,et al. Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .
[25] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Letter position information and printed word perception: the relative-position priming constraint. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.