Exploring the Behaviour of Distracted Drivers during Different Levels of Automation in Driving

Increased levels of automation in driving can reduce drivers’ situation-awareness and cause erratic changes to workload and skills degradation following prolonged exposure. In addition, drivers (particularly those who are vulnerable to the onset of boredom/fatigue) may engage in non-driving related, and potentially distracting, secondary tasks. Understanding the behavioural cues associated with this change in driver state can assist in the design and development of future driver monitoring systems that intervene in instances where a driver exhibits ‘high’ levels of distraction. The aim of this study was to explore the behavioural cues associated with distraction caused by a non-driving related secondary task (pseudo-text reading) during manual, partially-automated and highly-automated driving in a mediumfidelity driving simulator. Results from thirty drivers show that highly-automated driving was characterised by reduced workload, increased secondary task times and longer in-vehicle glances, compared to manual and partially-automated driving. In contrast, partially-automated driving was characterised by high workload, poor secondary task performance and low levels of situation awareness. Furthermore, primary and secondary task performance immediately following take-over during partiallyautomated driving was significantly compromised. The results indicate that the same type of ‘distraction’ can elicit different behavioural cues depending upon the level of automation within driving. This information can be used to further the development of future driver monitoring systems.

[1]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Pupillary, heart rate, and skin resistance changes during a mental task. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  A. James McKnight,et al.  Driver Education Task Analysis. Volume I: Task Descriptions. Final Report (August 1969-July 1970). , 1970 .

[3]  L. Bainbridge Ironies of Automation , 1982 .

[4]  S. Hart,et al.  Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .

[5]  D A Norman,et al.  The 'problem' with automation: inappropriate feedback and interaction, not 'over-automation'. , 1990, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[6]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Performance Consequences of Automation-Induced 'Complacency' , 1993 .

[7]  Mica R. Endsley,et al.  Toward a Theory of Situation Awareness in Dynamic Systems , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[8]  Mica R. Endsley,et al.  The Out-of-the-Loop Performance Problem and Level of Control in Automation , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[9]  Neville A. Stanton,et al.  From fly-by-wire to drive-by-wire: Safety implications of automation in vehicles , 1996 .

[10]  F Saad,et al.  ASSESSING NEW DRIVING SUPPORT SYSTEMS: CONTRIBUTION OF AN ANALYSIS OF DRIVERS' ACTIVITY IN REAL SITUATIONS , 1996 .

[11]  David B. Kaber,et al.  Out‐of‐the‐loop performance problems and the use of intermediate levels of automation for improved control system functioning and safety , 1997 .

[12]  Jaj Jacques Roufs,et al.  Text quality metrics for visual display units:: I. Methodological aspects , 1997 .

[13]  D. E Haigney,et al.  Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance: task demand characteristics and compensatory processes , 2000 .

[14]  Michael J. Goodman,et al.  NHTSA DRIVER DISTRACTION RESEARCH: PAST, PRESENT, AND FUTURE , 2001 .

[15]  Hargutt,et al.  EYELID MOVEMENTS AND THEIR PREDICTIVE VALUE FOR FATIGUE STAGES , 2001 .

[16]  A. Jameson Adaptive interfaces and agents , 2002 .

[17]  Mark S. Young,et al.  Malleable Attentional Resources Theory: A New Explanation for the Effects of Mental Underload on Performance , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[18]  David B. Kaber,et al.  The effects of level of automation and adaptive automation on human performance, situation awareness and workload in a dynamic control task , 2004 .

[19]  John D. Lee,et al.  Trust in Automation: Designing for Appropriate Reliance , 2004 .

[20]  Gary Burnett,et al.  Defining Driver Distraction , 2005 .

[21]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Surrogate in-vehicle information systems and driver behaviour: effects of visual and cognitive load in simulated rural driving , 2005 .

[22]  Thomas A. Dingus,et al.  The 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study Phase II – Results of the 100-Car Field Experiment , 2006 .

[23]  N A Stanton,et al.  What's skill got to do with it? Vehicle automation and driver mental workload , 2007, Ergonomics.

[24]  Katja Kircher,et al.  Driver distraction : a review of the literature , 2007 .

[25]  Michelle L. Reyes,et al.  Effects of cognitive load presence and duration on driver eye movements and event detection performance , 2008 .

[26]  C. Wickens,et al.  Situation Awareness, Mental Workload, and Trust in Automation: Viable, Empirically Supported Cognitive Engineering Constructs , 2008 .

[27]  Ralph H. Craft,et al.  Driver distraction and inattention in the USA large truck and national motor vehicle crash causation studies , 2009 .

[28]  Serge Boverie,et al.  The Importance of Driver State Assessment Within Highly Automated Vehicles , 2009 .

[29]  John D Lee,et al.  Combining cognitive and visual distraction: less than the sum of its parts. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[30]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Control Task Substitution in Semiautomated Driving , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[31]  O. Carsten,et al.  Mitigating the effects of in-vehicle distractions through use of the Psychological Refractory Period paradigm. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[32]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Behavioural changes in drivers experiencing highly-automated vehicle control in varying traffic conditions , 2013 .

[33]  Amanda N Stephens,et al.  Multiple driver distractions: a systemic transport problem. , 2015, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[34]  Klaus Bengler,et al.  Driver State Monitoring Systems-- Transferable Knowledge Manual Driving to HAD , 2015 .

[35]  N Merat,et al.  Leading to distraction: Driver distraction, lead car, and road environment. , 2016, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[36]  David R. Large,et al.  A driving simulator study to explore the effects of text size on the visual demand of in-vehicle displays , 2016, Displays.

[37]  David B. Kaber,et al.  The effect of driver cognitive abilities and distractions on situation awareness and performance under hazard conditions , 2016 .

[38]  Halszka Jarodzka,et al.  Before your very eyes: the value and limitations of eye tracking in medical education , 2017, Medical education.

[39]  R. M. Taylor,et al.  Situational Awareness Rating Technique (Sart): The Development of a Tool for Aircrew Systems Design , 2017 .