Months in space: Synaesthesia modulates attention and action

Month–space synaesthetes experience months as sequences arranged in spatially defined configurations. While most works on synaesthesia have studied its perceptual implications, this study focuses on the synaesthetic influence on a synaesthete's action behaviour. S.M., a month–space synaesthete, and 5 matched controls performed a spatial Stroop-like task in a haptics and virtual reality combined environment, which was especially designed to simulate S.M.'s three-dimensional synaesthetic experience. In the experiment, a circle and a word were presented simultaneously. The word consisted of either a month name or a direction name and was located at the centre of the screen, while the circle was displayed in one of four peripheral positions—top, bottom, right, or left. When S.M. was asked to ignore the word and reach for the circle, no effects were found. In contrast, when she was asked to ignore the circle and reach for a location indicated by the word, a congruency effect was found for both months and direction words. Crucially, these effects were evident in all measurements of reaching performance (i.e., path, velocity, and trajectory of movement). Our findings revealed that for month–space synaesthetes, months trigger spatial shifts of attention in a similar manner as directions do. Moreover, these shifts of attention affected not only latent cognitive processes (i.e., reaction time) but also overt behaviour (i.e., entire hand movements).

[1]  R. C. Kadosh,et al.  Exceptional Abilities in the Spatial Representation of Numbers and Time , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[2]  R. C. Kadosh,et al.  Synesthesia: Gluing together time, number and space , 2011 .

[3]  J. Simner,et al.  A foundation for savantism? Visuo-spatial synaesthetes present with cognitive benefits , 2009, Cortex.

[4]  D. Eagleman The objectification of overlearned sequences: A new view of spatial sequence synesthesia , 2009, Cortex.

[5]  D. Smilek,et al.  A different outlook on time: Visual and auditory month names elicit different mental vantage points for a time-space synaesthete , 2009, Cortex.

[6]  Mark C. Price Spatial forms and mental imagery , 2009, Cortex.

[7]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Time–space synaesthesia – A cognitive advantage? , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[8]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  A. Henik,et al.  When 9 is not on the right: Implications from number-form synesthesia , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[10]  D. Eagleman,et al.  Wednesday Is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia , 2009 .

[11]  R. Mentzoni,et al.  Where is January? The month-SNARC effect in sequence-form synaesthetes , 2008, Cortex.

[12]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Target selection in visual search as revealed by movement trajectories , 2008, Vision Research.

[13]  E. Milán,et al.  Synaesthesia: The existing state of affairs , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  D. Smilek,et al.  Ovals of time: Time-space associations in synaesthesia , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[15]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Can synaesthesia research inform cognitive science? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Synaesthesia: an Overview of Contemporary Findings and Controversies , 2006, Cortex.

[17]  N. Sagiv,et al.  What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms? , 2006, Cognition.

[18]  R. Rafal,et al.  Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  Wim Fias,et al.  The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized , 2003, Cognition.

[20]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic Control of Visual Attention , 2001, Psychological science.

[21]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  W. Schneider VAM: A neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action , 1995 .

[23]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  P. Seymour Internal representation of the months , 1980 .

[25]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[26]  David R. Olson,et al.  Spatial and verbal rivalry in a Stroop-like task. , 1975 .

[27]  Philip H. K. Seymour,et al.  Stroop interference in naming and verifying spatial locations , 1973 .

[28]  R. Proctor,et al.  Selective Visual Attention , 2004 .

[29]  A. Henik,et al.  Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .

[30]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[32]  S. B. Vincent The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat , 1912 .