The Role of Numbers and Statistics within Conversation Analysis

In this article, I discuss the role of numbers and statistics within Conversation Analysis (hereafter CA). Due to space limitations, I do not review CA (see Heritage, 1984). Furthermore, I do not focus on CA’s concerns regarding the use of quantification (e.g., ratios) to derive or operationalize statistically manipulable variables (see Schegloff, 1993). Finally, I do not focus on CA’s concerns regarding the validity of statistically associating variables, especially when they are not demonstrably relevant to, or procedurally consequential for, participants (see Schegloff, 1992). The latter enterprise is sometimes called applied CA when one of the variables is a product of CA. However, applied CA is a misnomer because it implies that such work represents a type of CA, which it does not. Rather, it represents an application of CA (which takes nothing away from its value; see Heritage, 2004). Instead, I address numbers and statistics in terms of proving certain types of CA claims, with the hope of clarifying ambiguity and increasing analytic rigor. I begin by reviewing CA’s conception of order and then outline two different, yet interrelated, CA projects: analyzing single cases and documenting practices of action. The latter project involves claims about communication rules that generate regular patterns of understanding and interactional organization. As such, achieving this goal involves evidentiary requirements that implicate numbers and statistics. Conversation analysts should (perhaps reluctantly) find these requirements to be methodologically acceptable, although they are neither easily nor typically satisfied, at least in single articles/chapters. COMMUNICATION METHODS AND MEASURES, 1(1), 65–75 Copyright © 2007, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

[1]  S. Jacobs Evidence and Inference in Conversation Analysis , 1988 .

[2]  Geoffrey Raymond Grammar and Social Organization: Yes/No Interrogatives and the Structure of Responding , 2003, American Sociological Review.

[3]  Stuart J. Sigman,et al.  ON COMMUNICATION RULES FROM A SOCIAL PERSPECTIVE , 1980 .

[4]  Charles Goodwin,et al.  Structures of Social Action: Notes on story structure and the organization of participation , 1985 .

[5]  Gene H. Lerner Selecting next speaker: The context-sensitive operation of a context-free organization , 2003, Language in Society.

[6]  E. Schegloff Reflections on Quantification in the Study of Conversation , 1993 .

[7]  Emanuel A. Schegloff,et al.  On an Actual Virtual Servo-Mechanism for Guessing Bad News: A Single Case Conjecture , 1988 .

[8]  Bernard J. Frieden,et al.  Notes on Methodology , 2020, Immigrant Incorporation in East Asian Democracies.

[9]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  Emanuel A. Schegloff,et al.  Practices and actions: Boundary cases of other‐initiated repair , 1997 .

[11]  R. Mackay Garfinkel and Ethnomethodology , 1987 .

[12]  Herbert S. Gross,et al.  Therapeutic Discourse: Psychotherapy as Conversation , 1978 .

[13]  E. Schegloff Analyzing Single Episodes of Interaction: An Exercise in Conversation Analysis , 1987 .

[14]  J. Cappella The method of proof by example in interaction analysis , 1990 .

[15]  Jeffrey D. Robinson,et al.  A preference for progressivity in interaction , 2006, Language in Society.

[16]  Gail Jefferson,et al.  Notes on a systematic deployment of the acknowledgement tokens “Yeah”; and “Mm Hm”; , 1984 .

[17]  Emanuel A. Schegloff,et al.  Confirming Allusions: Toward an Empirical Account of Action , 1996, American Journal of Sociology.