Coherence thresholds for discrimination of motion direction in infants

The sensitivity of 3-month-old infants to direction of motion in random-dot patterns was assessed by measuring coherence thresholds for the discrimination of a pattern, in which opposite directions were segregated into alternate horizontal strips, from an unsegregated pattern. The coherently moving dots had a displacement size of 0.16 deg (velocity 8 deg/sec), and their direction of motion reversed periodically. For both infants and an adult subject coherence thresholds decreased with increasing height of the segregated strips, and with increasing duration of the interval between direction reversals. However the infants required larger minimum heights and longer minimum durations in order to extract motion direction. Even under the best conditions infants were markedly less sensitive, with coherence thresholds of around 50%, compared with 5-7% for the adult. In addition, within the group of infants coherence thresholds were negatively correlated with age. This developmental increase in motion sensitivity at an intermediate velocity suggests that a large part of the improvement in upper and lower velocity thresholds during development is a result of a uniform increase in sensitivity across all velocities, though the results do not rule out additional specific improvements in sensitivity at the extremes of the velocity range.

[1]  S. McKee,et al.  Sequential recruitment in the discrimination of velocity. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[2]  J. Koenderink,et al.  The distribution of human motion detector properties in the monocular visual field , 1986, Vision Research.

[3]  D. Teller The forced-choice preferential looking procedure: A psychophysical technique for use with human infants. , 1979 .

[4]  William H. Swanson,et al.  Infant spatiotemporal vision: Dependence of spatial contrast sensitivity on temporal frequency , 1990, Vision Research.

[5]  O. Braddick,et al.  The combination of motion signals over time , 1989, Vision Research.

[6]  Richard N. Aslin,et al.  Oculomotor responses to step-ramp targets by young human infants , 1990, Vision Research.

[7]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Spatiotemporal integration in the detection of coherent motion , 1984, Vision Research.

[8]  C. Baker,et al.  The basis of area and dot number effects in random dot motion perception , 1982, Vision Research.

[9]  O. J. Braddick,et al.  Extension of displacement limits in multiple-exposure sequences of apparent motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[10]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[11]  J. Dannemiller,et al.  The detection of slow stimulus movement in 2- to 5-month-olds. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  R. Aslin,et al.  Velocity thresholds in human infants : implications for the perception of motion , 1990 .

[13]  F. C. Volkmann,et al.  Infant responses of ocular fixation to moving visual stimuli. , 1976, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  G. B. Wetherill,et al.  SEQUENTIAL ESTIMATION OF POINTS ON A PSYCHOMETRIC FUNCTION. , 1965, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[15]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  Development of motion-specific cortical responses in infancy , 1991, Vision Research.

[16]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  The development of maximum displacement limits for discrimination of motion direction in infancy , 1992, Vision Research.

[17]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.