The point of no return in planar hand movements: an indication of the existence of high level motion primitives
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Kendall. Statistical Methods for Research Workers , 1937, Nature.
[2] P. Ladefoged,et al. Interruptibility of Speech , 1970 .
[3] H. Levitt. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[4] P Ladefoged,et al. Letter: Interruptibility of speech. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[5] M. Hallett,et al. Ballistic flexion movements of the human thumb. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.
[6] G. Logan. On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .
[7] J. Bachorowski,et al. Impulsivity in adults: Motor inhibition and time-interval estimation. , 1985 .
[8] D. Meyer,et al. The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] Gordon D. Logan,et al. Effects of methylphenidate on inhibitory control in hyperactive children , 1989, Journal of abnormal child psychology.
[10] J. Cooke,et al. Movement-related phasic muscle activation. I. Relations with temporal profile of movement. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] G. Logan,et al. In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] S Kornblum,et al. Does motor programming necessitate response execution? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] J. Bachorowski,et al. Impulsive motor behavior: effects of personality and goal salience. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[14] T. Carr,et al. Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .
[15] G D Logan,et al. Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] T. Flash,et al. Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] J. Sergeant,et al. Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: A biologically based model of child psychopathology , 1996, Journal of abnormal child psychology.
[18] I M Franks,et al. A horse race between independent processes: evidence for a phantom point of no return in preparation of a speeded motor response. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Jaap Oosterlaan,et al. Inhibitory dysfunction in hyperactive boys , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.
[20] G. Logan,et al. Development of inhibitory control across the life span. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[21] G. Band,et al. Inhibitory motor control in stop paradigms: review and reinterpretation of neural mechanisms. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[22] Rosemary Tannock,et al. Inhibitory Control Differences Following Mild Head Injury , 1999, Brain and Cognition.
[23] I M Franks,et al. Against a final ballistic process in the control of voluntary action: evidence using the Hoffmann reflex. , 2000, Motor control.
[24] Gordon D. Logan,et al. Confirmation of an Inhibitory Control Deficit in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2000, Journal of abnormal child psychology.
[25] I M Franks,et al. Inhibitory motor control in stop paradigms: comment on Band and van Boxtel (1999). , 2000, Acta psychologica.
[26] Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al. Inhibitory motor control in stop paradigms : reply to McGarry and Franks (2000) , 2000 .
[27] B. Pennington,et al. A Comparison of the Neuropsychological Profiles of the DSM-IV Subtypes of ADHD , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.
[28] G. Glover,et al. Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.
[29] M. W. Molen,et al. A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.
[30] David Zipser,et al. Reaching to grasp with a multi-jointed arm. I. Computational model. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[31] Sharon Morein-Zamir,et al. The Effect of Methylphenidate on Three Forms of Response Inhibition in Boys with AD/HD , 2003, Journal of abnormal child psychology.
[32] Ian M Franks,et al. Stopping and Restarting an Unfolding Action at Various Times , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[33] T. Robbins,et al. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[34] T. Flash,et al. When practice leads to co-articulation: the evolution of geometrically defined movement primitives , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Individual stopping times and cognitive control: converging evidence for the stop signal task from a continuous tracking paradigm. , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[36] M. Rieger,et al. Inhibition of ongoing responses following frontal, nonfrontal, and basal ganglia lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.
[37] D. Munoz,et al. Inhibitory control of eye movements during oculomotor countermanding in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[38] Katya Rubia,et al. Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection , 2003, NeuroImage.
[39] J. D. Cooke,et al. Initial agonist burst duration changes with movement amplitude in a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[40] J. Cooke,et al. Movement-related phasic muscle activation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] D. M. MacKay. Do ‘evaluation potentials’ reflect cognitive assessment? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] J. Cooke,et al. Initial agonist burst duration depends on movement amplitude , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[43] Romeo Chua,et al. Inhibiting prepared and ongoing responses: Is there more than one kind of stopping? , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[44] Sharon Morein-Zamir,et al. Executive functioning in boys with ADHD: primarily an inhibition deficit? , 2004, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[45] Robert Licht,et al. Effects of stop signal modality, stop signal intensity and tracking method on inhibitory performance as determined by use of the stop signal paradigm. , 2005, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[46] J. Kenemans,et al. The pure electrophysiology of stopping. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[47] Daniel H. Mathalon,et al. Fore-period effect and stop-signal reaction time , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[48] M. Brammer,et al. Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.
[49] Eran Stark,et al. Partial cross-correlation analysis resolves ambiguity in the encoding of multiple movement features. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[50] Elizabeth Torres,et al. Space-time separation during obstacle-avoidance learning in monkeys. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[51] Neeraj J Gandhi,et al. Behavioral evaluation of movement cancellation. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[52] Tamar Flash,et al. The acquisition and implementation of the smoothness maximization motion strategy is dependent on spatial accuracy demands , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[53] Pierpaolo Pani,et al. Inhibitory control of reaching movements in humans , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.