Acoustic and Temporal Variation in Gelada (Theropithecus gelada) Loud Calls Advertise Male Quality
暂无分享,去创建一个
Julia Fischer | Thore J. Bergman | J. Beehner | T. Bergman | J. Fischer | Marcela E. Benítez | Jacinta C. Beehner | Aliza le Roux | A. Roux
[1] J. Altmann,et al. Corrigendum to “Testosterone related to age and life-history stages in male baboons and geladas” [Horm. Behav. 56/4 (2009) 472–480] , 2016, Hormones and Behavior.
[2] Marla M. Holt,et al. Vocal performance affects metabolic rate in dolphins: implications for animals communicating in noisy environments , 2015, The Journal of Experimental Biology.
[3] T. Bergman,et al. The socio‐genetics of a complex society: female gelada relatedness patterns mirror association patterns in a multilevel society , 2014, Molecular ecology.
[4] D. Mennill,et al. Vocal behavior of black‐fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons): Acoustic properties and behavioral contexts of loud calls , 2014, American journal of primatology.
[5] K. Hammerschmidt,et al. Individual, Contextual, and Age-Related Acoustic Variation in Simakobu (Simias concolor) Loud Calls , 2013, PloS one.
[6] K. Hammerschmidt,et al. Cues to Androgens and Quality in Male Gibbon Songs , 2013, PloS one.
[7] T. Bergman,et al. Social Knowledge and Signals in Primates , 2013, American journal of primatology.
[8] JULIA FISCHER,et al. Bioacoustic Field Research: A Primer to Acoustic Analyses and Playback Experiments With Primates , 2013, American journal of primatology.
[9] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Acoustic variability and individual distinctiveness in the vocal repertoire of red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.
[10] T. Bergman,et al. Indirect rival assessment in a social primate, Theropithecus gelada , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[11] T. Bergman,et al. Experimental evidence for limited vocal recognition in a wild primate: implications for the social complexity hypothesis , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[12] Ingo R. Titze,et al. A Cervid Vocal Fold Model Suggests Greater Glottal Efficiency in Calling at High Frequencies , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[13] Serge A. Wich,et al. Acoustic Properties of Long Calls Given by Flanged Male Orang-Utans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) Reflect Both Individual Identity and Context , 2010 .
[14] Robin I. M. Dunbar. Structure of Gelada Baboon Reproductive Units: IV. Integration at Group Level , 2010 .
[15] K. Hammerschmidt,et al. Loud calls in male crested macaques, Macaca nigra: a signal of dominance in a tolerant species , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[16] J. Altmann,et al. Testosterone related to age and life-history stages in male baboons and geladas , 2009, Hormones and Behavior.
[17] E. Zimmermann,et al. Geographic variation in loud calls of sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ssp.) and their implications for conservation , 2008, American journal of primatology.
[18] Roger Mundry,et al. Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative , 2007, Animal Behaviour.
[19] Tobias Riede,et al. Nonlinear acoustics in the pant hoots of common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): vocalizing at the edge. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[20] J. Fischer,et al. Do age- and sex-related variations reliably reflect body size in non-human primate vocalizations? A review , 2007, Primates.
[21] J. Fischer,et al. Sounds and size: identification of acoustic variables that reflect body size in hamadryas baboons, Papio hamadryas , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[22] Roberto A. Delgado. Sexual Selection in the Loud Calls of Male Primates: Signal Content and Function , 2006, International Journal of Primatology.
[23] T. Clutton‐Brock,et al. Red deer stags use formants as assessment cues during intrasexual agonistic interactions , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[24] S. Piertney,et al. Testosterone, immunocompetence, and honest sexual signaling in male red grouse , 2004 .
[25] R. Seyfarth,et al. Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[26] S. Wich,et al. Life-Phase Related Changes in Male Loud Call Characteristics and Testosterone Levels in Wild Thomas Langurs , 2003, International Journal of Primatology.
[27] P. Slater,et al. The energy cost of song in the canary, Serinus canaria , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[28] R. Seyfarth,et al. Loud calls as indicators of dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[29] R. Thomas,et al. The costs of singing in nightingales , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[30] Julia Fischer,et al. Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: influences of context, age, and individuality. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[31] Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton,et al. Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication , 2001 .
[32] H. Carl Gerhardt,et al. Female preference functions based on call duration in the gray tree frog (Hyla versicolor) , 2000 .
[33] JULIA FISCHER,et al. Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often leads to incorrectPvalues: examples fromAnimal Behaviour , 1998, Animal Behaviour.
[34] W. Fitch. Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[35] William E. Wagner. Deceptive or honest signalling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in call dominant frequency by male cricket frogs , 1992, Animal Behaviour.
[36] K. McComb. Female choice for high roaring rates in red deer, Cervus elaphus , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[37] Robin I. M. Dunbar. Structure of gelada baboon reproductive units. II. Social relationships between reproductive females , 1983, Animal Behaviour.
[38] R. Wrangham,et al. Ecological and Sociological Studies of Gelada Baboons , 1980 .
[39] A. Zahavi. Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. , 1975, Journal of theoretical biology.
[40] Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al. Social dynamics of gelada baboons. , 1975, Contributions to primatology.
[41] H. Schuckman. Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. , 1966 .
[42] P. Pye-Smith. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex , 1871, Nature.
[43] Michael Tomasello,et al. Primate Cognition , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[44] Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al. Social organization of gelada baboons: Social units and definitions , 2006, Primates.
[45] W. Fitch,et al. Vocal production in nonhuman primates: Acoustics, physiology, and functional constraints on “honest” advertisement , 1995, American journal of primatology.
[46] E. Zimmermann,et al. Vocalizations of adult gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada): acoustic structure and behavioural context. , 1990, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.
[47] M. Inoue. Age gradations in vocalization and body weight in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). , 1988, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.
[48] Robin I. M. Dunbar. Reproductive Decisions: An Economic Analysis of Gelada Baboon Social Strategies , 1984 .
[49] U. Mori. Social structure of gelada baboons. , 1979, Contributions to primatology.
[50] U. Mori. Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons. Unit formation and the emergence of a new leader. , 1979, Contributions to primatology.
[51] I. Devore,et al. Baboon Social Behavior , 1965 .