Spatial Memory for Highly Familiar Environments Julia Frankenstein (julia.frankenstein@tuebingen.mpg.de) Tobias Meilinger (tobias.meilinger@tuebingen.mpg.de) Betty J. Mohler (betty.mohler@tuebingen.mpg.de) Heinrich H. Bulthoff (heinrich.buelthoff@tuebingen.mpg.de) Max-Planck-Institute for Biological Cybernetics Spemannstr. 38, 72076 Tubingen, Germany Abstract In this experiment we examined orientation dependency in human memory for a highly familiar environmental space. Twenty-seven inhabitants living for at least two years in Tubingen saw a photorealistic virtual model of the city center (Virtual Tubingen) through a head-mounted display. They were teleported to five different initial locations in Virtual Tubingen and asked to point towards well-known target locations. This procedure was repeated in twelve different body-orientations for each of the initial locations. Participants pointed more accurately when oriented northwards regardless of the initial location. We also found a small effect of local orientation. The more participants were aligned with the street leading to the target location the better was their pointing performance. Even though the strong alignment effect with a global orientation is predicted by reference direction theory, this theory does not predict that this global orientation is, first, common for almost all participants, and second, that this orientation is north. We discuss our results with respect to well-known theories of spatial memory and speculate that the bias we find for north orientation is due to participants relying on memory of a city map of Tubingen for their pointing response. Keywords: spatial memory; reference frame; environmental space; reference direction; view dependent; orientation independent; alignment; map Introduction In a familiar environment our spatial memory is needed for us to perform daily tasks. We must remember where our work is located, the route to travel there from our home, and we also need to be able to calculate a detour on occasions when our route is obstructed. A lot of spatial processes likely require a mental representation of our environment. The question addressed in this paper is how highly familiar environments are represented in memory. Theories of Spatial Memory There are many theoretical positions concerning the organization of spatial memory (e.g., Burgess, 2006; Gallistel, 1990; O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Rump & McNamara, 2007; Wang & Spelke, 2002). Most theoretical positions can be categorized with regard to their prediction of whether humans store their environment orientation dependent with respect to one or more reference directions, orientation dependent with respect to experienced views, and/or orientation independent. Reference Direction Theory. Reference direction theory states that humans store their spatial knowledge of the environment with respect to one or more reference directions. This theoretical position is mainly supported by McNamara and colleagues (e.g., Kelly, McNamara, Bodenheimer, Carr & Rieser, 2008; Mou, Xiao & McNamara, 2008; Shelton & McNamara, 2001; Rump & McNamara, 2007). The theory states that when learning a new environment, its spatial structure is stored in the specific orientation of a reference direction, whose orientation is determined by either the initial exposure to this environment (e.g., the first view of a room), or by its most salient orientation (intrinsic axis, e.g., parallel to the longer walls of a rectangular room). Initially reference direction theory was only tested in vista spaces. Vista spaces are spaces which could be explored from one point of view, e.g., one single room (see Montello 1993). Spaces such as our city of residence are considered environmental spaces. This kind of space could not be explored from one single point of view, by definition it contains at least two vista spaces which need to be integrated. McNamara, Slucenski and Rump (2008) extended the reference direction theory for environmental spaces, proposing hierarchical reference systems at different scales. Higher-order reference systems (e.g., for a city district) define the spatial relations between lower-order reference systems (e.g., for single streets). By learning an environmental space, humans encode multiple local reference systems with local reference directions. These local reference systems become part of a higher-order reference system by aligning their local reference directions to a single reference direction common for the whole environment. This global reference direction likely differs between individuals, i.e., there is no a priori reason to assume that all individuals should necessarily come up with the same global reference direction. According to this theoretical approach, humans should perform spatial tasks within a highly familiar environment best when they are aligned with the reference direction they use to store the whole environmental space. View Dependent Memory. Theories proposing a view dependent memory assume that all parts of the environment are stored in the local orientation in which they were explored (e.g., Cartwright & Collett, 1983; Christou & Bulthoff, 1999; Gillner, Weis & Mallot, 2008; Simons & Wang, 1998; Trullier, Wiener, Berthoz & Meyer, 1997;
[1]
R. D. Easton,et al.
Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge.
,
1995,
Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[2]
Irwin N. Jankovic,et al.
Principles of spatial problem solving.
,
1982
.
[3]
T. McNamara,et al.
Human Spatial Memory and Navigation
,
2008
.
[4]
Andrew U. Frank,et al.
Spatial Information Theory A Theoretical Basis for GIS
,
1993,
Lecture Notes in Computer Science.
[5]
A. Miyake,et al.
The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking
,
2005
.
[6]
Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.
Perceiving Real-World Viewpoint Changes
,
1998
.
[7]
M. Sholl,et al.
Allocentric coding of object-to-object relations in overlearned and novel environments.
,
2005,
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.
[8]
T. S. Collett,et al.
Landmark learning in bees
,
1983,
Journal of comparative physiology.
[9]
N. Franklin,et al.
Parsing surrounding space into regions
,
1995,
Memory & cognition.
[10]
Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.
Visual homing in the absence of feature-based landmark information
,
2008,
Cognition.
[11]
Daniel R. Montello,et al.
Scale and Multiple Psychologies of Space
,
1993,
COSIT.
[12]
Daniel R. Montello,et al.
Spatial Information Theory A Theoretical Basis for GIS
,
1995,
Lecture Notes in Computer Science.
[13]
Johanna D. Moore,et al.
Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society
,
2005
.
[14]
R. Passingham.
The hippocampus as a cognitive map
J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00
,
1979,
Neuroscience.
[15]
S. Becker,et al.
Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery.
,
2007,
Psychological review.
[16]
Wilfried Brauer,et al.
Spatial Cognition III
,
2003,
Lecture Notes in Computer Science.
[17]
N. Burgess,et al.
Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine
,
2006,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[18]
E. Spelke,et al.
Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals
,
2002
.
[19]
M. Jeanne Sholl,et al.
The Role of a Self-Reference System in Spatial Navigation
,
2001,
COSIT.
[20]
H. H. Bülthoff,et al.
Schätzen von Richtungen in realen und virtuellen Umgebungen
,
1998
.
[21]
Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.
Recently learned environmental spaces are represented as interconnected vista spaces
,
2011
.
[22]
Barbara Tversky,et al.
Cognitive Maps, Cognitive Collages, and Spatial Mental Models
,
1993,
COSIT.
[23]
C. Gallistel.
The organization of learning
,
1990
.
[24]
Tobias Meilinger,et al.
The Network of Reference Frames Theory: A Synthesis of Graphs and Cognitive Maps
,
2008,
Spatial Cognition.
[25]
H. Bülthoff,et al.
View dependence in scene recognition after active learning
,
1999,
Memory & cognition.
[26]
H. Bülthoff,et al.
Orientation Specificity in Long-Term-Memory for Environmental Spaces
,
2007
.
[27]
Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.
The shape of human navigation: How environmental geometry is used in maintenance of spatial orientation
,
2008,
Cognition.
[28]
Weimin Mou,et al.
Reference directions and reference objects in spatial memory of a briefly viewed layout
,
2008,
Cognition.
[29]
Timothy P. McNamara,et al.
Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory
,
2001,
Cognitive Psychology.
[30]
Jean-Arcady Meyer,et al.
BIOLOGICALLY BASED ARTIFICIAL NAVIGATION SYSTEMS: REVIEW AND PROSPECTS
,
1997,
Progress in Neurobiology.
[31]
Christopher D. Wickens,et al.
The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking: Design Applications of Visual Spatial Thinking: The Importance of Frame of Reference
,
2005
.
[32]
H. Roediger.
Cognitive psychology of memory
,
2008
.
[33]
Timothy P. McNamara,et al.
Updating in Models of Spatial Memory
,
2006,
Spatial Cognition.
[34]
Dylan M. Jones,et al.
Navigating Buildings in "Desk-Top" Virtual Environments: Experimental Investigations Using Extended Navigational Experience
,
1997
.