PII: S0042-6989(97)00422-7

When viewing a moving object, details may appear blurred if the object’s motion is not compensated for by the eyes. Smooth pursuit is a voluntary eye movement that is used to stabilize a moving object. Most studies of smooth pursuit have used small, foveal targets as stimuli (e.g. Lisberger SG and Westbrook LE. J Neurosci 1985;5:1662–1673.). However, in the laboratory, smooth pursuit is poorer when a small object is tracked across a background, presumably due to a conflict between the primitive optokinetic reflex and smooth pursuit. Functionally, this could occur if the motion signal arising from the target and its surroundings were averaged, resulting in a smaller net motion signal. We asked if the smooth pursuit system could spatially summate coherent motion, i.e. if its response would improve when motion in the peripheral retina was in the same direction as motion in the fovea. Observers tracked random-dot cinematograms (RDC) which were devoid of consistent position cues to isolate the motion response. Either the height or the density of the display was systematically varied. Eye speed at the end of the open-loop period was greater for cinematograms than for a single spot. In addition, eye acceleration increased and latency decreased as the size of the aperture increased. Changes in the density produced similar but smaller effects on both acceleration and latency. The improved pursuit for larger motion stimuli suggests that neuronal mechanisms subserving smooth pursuit spatially average motion information to obtain a stronger motion signal. © 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

[1]  D J Heeger,et al.  Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[2]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[4]  P. Verghese,et al.  Combining speed information across space , 1995, Vision Research.

[5]  H. Noda,et al.  Responses of Purkinje cells and mossy fibres in the flocculus of the monkey during sinusoidal movements of a visual pattern. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Visual responses of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar flocculus during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. I. Simple spikes. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—III. Guessing unpredictable target displacements , 1981, Vision Research.

[9]  J. D. Mollon,et al.  Control of eye movements , 1977, Nature.

[10]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Target position and velocity: The stimuli for smooth pursuit eye movements , 1980, Vision Research.

[12]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[13]  A. Yuille,et al.  A common theoretical framework for visual motion's spatial and temporal coherence , 1989, [1989] Proceedings. Workshop on Visual Motion.

[14]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  S. McKee,et al.  Visual acuity in the presence of retinal-image motion. , 1975, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Visual motion processing for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  D. A. Suzuki,et al.  The role of the posterior vermis of monkey cerebellum in smooth-pursuit eye movement control. II. Target velocity-related Purkinje cell activity. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[20]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Quantitative analysis of visual receptive fields of neurons in nucleus of the optic tract and dorsal terminal nucleus of the accessory optic tract in macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  B. Cohen,et al.  Quantitative analysis of the velocity characteristics of optokinetic nystagmus and optokinetic after‐nystagmus , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Vector Averaging for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Initiated by Two Moving Targets in Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  F A Miles,et al.  Effects of stationary textured backgrounds on the initiation of pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Psychophysics of Motion Perception , 1982 .

[25]  R D Yee,et al.  Effects of an optokinetic background on pursuit eye movements. , 1983, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[26]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Response properties of dorsolateral pontine units during smooth pursuit in the rhesus macaque. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. II. Differentiation of retinal from extraretinal inputs. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al.  A computational theory for the perception of coherent visual motion , 1988, Nature.

[29]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  K. N. GURNEY,et al.  A Model for the Spatial Integration and Differentiation of Velocity Signals , 1996, Vision Research.

[32]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Smooth-pursuit initiation in the presence of a textured background in monkey , 1986, Vision Research.

[33]  H. Collewijn Optokinetic eye movements in the rabbit: input-output relations. , 1969, Vision research.

[34]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .

[35]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Human smooth and saccadic eye movements during voluntary pursuit of different target motions on different backgrounds. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[36]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity , 1987, Vision Research.

[37]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Spatiotemporal integration in the detection of coherent motion , 1984, Vision Research.

[38]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Spatial and temporal parameters of motion detection in the peripheral visual field. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[39]  Robert M. Steinman,et al.  Voluntary control of smooth pursuit velocity , 1969 .

[40]  Effects of eye and head position on horizontal and vertical smooth pursuit. , 1997, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[41]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Cortical visual motion processing for oculomotor control. , 1990, Research publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.

[42]  S. McKee,et al.  Temporal coherence theory for the detection and measurement of visual motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[43]  T. Poggio,et al.  A parallel algorithm for real-time computation of optical flow , 1989, Nature.

[44]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Active and passive smooth eye movements: Effects of stimulus size and location , 1985, Vision Research.

[45]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Generation of smooth-pursuit eye movements: neuronal mechanisms and pathways , 1991, Neuroscience Research.