Sound source location modulates the irrelevant-sound effect

Participants memorized lists of visually presented digits in silence or while ignoring distractor sounds that either came from the front and thus from the direction in which participants’ attention was oriented, or from behind. Distractor sounds impaired recall performance, but the largest impairment was observed when the sound source was directionally close to the frontal visual target display. The results are consistent with the assumption of cross-modal attentional links in models of attention, and they are problematic for explanations of the irrelevant-sound effect within working memory models that do not specify an explicit role of attention in the maintenance of information for immediate serial recall.

[1]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Nick Mosdell,et al.  The role of habituation in the disruption of recall performance by irrelevant sound , 1997 .

[3]  J Driver,et al.  Cross-modal selective attention: On the difficulty of ignoring sounds at the locus of visual attention , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  A. Miyake,et al.  Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control , 1999 .

[5]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Objects, streams, and threads of auditory attention. , 1993 .

[6]  Bill Macken,et al.  Role of Habituation in the Irrelevant Sound Effect : Evidence From the Effects of Token Set Size and Rate of Transition , 2001 .

[7]  Elke B. Lange Disruption of attention by irrelevant stimuli in serial recall , 2005 .

[8]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Interference from degraded auditory stimuli: linear effects of changing-state in the irrelevant sequence. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  Karl Christoph Klauer,et al.  Working memory and changing-state hypothesis. , 1999 .

[10]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech : Implications for the structure of working memory , 1982 .

[11]  W. Ellermeier,et al.  Is level irrelevant in "irrelevant speech"? Effects of loudness, signal-to-noise ratio, and binaural unmasking. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Tom Campbell,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of auditory distraction , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  I. Neath Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  E. Langendijk,et al.  Contribution of spectral cues to human sound localization. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  Spatial Effects in Speech Perception in the Absence of Spatial Competition , 1977, Perception.

[16]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Role of Habituation in the Irrelevant Sound Effect: Evidence From the Effects of Token Set Size and Rate of Transition , 1998 .

[17]  Herbert A. Colle,et al.  Auditory encoding in visual short-term recall: effects of noise intensity and spatial location , 1980 .

[18]  E. Schröger,et al.  ERP effects of intermodal attention and cross-modal links in spatial attention. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[19]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Disruption of proofreading by irrelevant speech: Effects of attention, arousal or memory? , 1990 .

[20]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[21]  L. M. Ward,et al.  On asymmetries in cross-modal spatial attention orienting , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  On the Irrelevance of Semantic Information for the “Irrelevant Speech” Effect , 1996 .

[23]  P Bertelson,et al.  Spatial Constraints on Focused Attention: Beyond the Right-Side Advantage , 1976, Perception.

[24]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Working Memory: The multiple-component model , 1999 .

[25]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory attentional capture during serial recall: violations at encoding of an algorithm-based neural model? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  F L Wightman,et al.  Localization using nonindividualized head-related transfer functions. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  Jon Driver,et al.  Crossmodal Spatial Attention: Evidence from Human Performance , 2004 .

[29]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  The N1 hypothesis and irrelevant sound: evidence from token set size effects. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[30]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect : Implications for phonological coding in working memory , 1993 .

[31]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Interference in memory by process or content? A reply to Neath (2000) , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Neural Substrates of Perceptual Enhancement by Cross-Modal Spatial Attention , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  D. C. Lecompte,et al.  The importance of semantic similarity to the irrelevant speech effect , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[34]  Susan E. Gathercole,et al.  Models Of Short-Term Memory , 1996 .

[35]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory Distraction and Short-Term Memory: Phenomena and Practical Implications , 2001, Hum. Factors.

[36]  A. Buchner,et al.  Artificially induced valence of distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[37]  Deborah M. Shaibe,et al.  On the Irrelevance of Phonological Similarity to the Irrelevant Speech Effect , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[38]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Valence of distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[39]  N. Cowan An embedded-processes model of working memory , 1999 .

[40]  J. S. Nairne A feature model of immediate memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[41]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[42]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[43]  C. P. Beaman,et al.  The object-oriented episodic record model , 1996 .

[44]  E. Elliott,et al.  The irrelevant-speech effect and children: theoretical implications of developmental change , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[45]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[46]  A. Sockloff,et al.  Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences: (revised edition), by Jacob Cohen. New York: Academic Press, 1977, xv + 474 pp., $24.50. , 1978 .

[47]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  Word frequency of irrelevant speech distractors affects serial recall , 2005, Memory & cognition.