Modeling conflict and error in the medial frontal cortex

Despite intensive study, the role of the dorsal medial frontal cortex (dMFC) in error monitoring and conflict processing remains actively debated. The current experiment manipulated conflict type (stimulus conflict only or stimulus and response selection conflict) and utilized a novel modeling approach to isolate error and conflict variance during a multimodal numeric Stroop task. Specifically, hemodynamic response functions resulting from two statistical models that either included or isolated variance arising from relatively few error trials were directly contrasted. Twenty‐four participants completed the task while undergoing event‐related functional magnetic resonance imaging on a 1.5‐Tesla scanner. Response times monotonically increased based on the presence of pure stimulus or stimulus and response selection conflict. Functional results indicated that dMFC activity was present during trials requiring response selection and inhibition of competing motor responses, but absent during trials involving pure stimulus conflict. A comparison of the different statistical models suggested that relatively few error trials contributed to a disproportionate amount of variance (i.e., activity) throughout the dMFC, but particularly within the rostral anterior cingulate gyrus (rACC). Finally, functional connectivity analyses indicated that an empirically derived seed in the dorsal ACC/pre‐SMA exhibited strong connectivity (i.e., positive correlation) with prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex but was anti‐correlated with the default‐mode network. An empirically derived seed from the rACC exhibited the opposite pattern, suggesting that sub‐regions of the dMFC exhibit different connectivity patterns with other large scale networks implicated in internal mentations such as daydreaming (default‐mode) versus the execution of top‐down attentional control (fronto‐parietal). Hum Brain Mapp, 2012. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex activity can be independent of response conflict in Stroop-like tasks , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[4]  W. Gehring,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex Activity and Loss-Related Responses to Errors , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Roesch,et al.  Neuronal activity in macaque SEF and ACC during performance of tasks involving conflict. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  A. Turken,et al.  Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  D. Harrington,et al.  The effects of stimulus modality and frequency of stimulus presentation on cross-modal distraction. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[10]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Connectivity-Based Parcellation of Human Cingulate Cortex and Its Relation to Functional Specialization , 2008 .

[12]  D. Tucker,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[14]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[15]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Are Arabic numerals processed as pictures in a Stroop interference task? , 2001, Psychological research.

[16]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[17]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[18]  H Garavan,et al.  A midline dissociation between error-processing and response-conflict monitoring , 2003, NeuroImage.

[19]  Katherine L. Roberts,et al.  Examining a Supramodal Network for Conflict Processing: A Systematic Review and Novel Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data for Related Visual and Auditory Stroop Tasks , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Kevin Murphy,et al.  The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced? , 2009, NeuroImage.

[21]  M. Fahle,et al.  The influence of response conflict on error processing: Evidence from event-related fMRI , 2008, Brain Research.

[22]  H Garavan,et al.  Artifactual fMRI group and condition differences driven by performance confounds , 2004, NeuroImage.

[23]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Error-likelihood prediction in the medial frontal cortex: a critical evaluation. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  M. Farah,et al.  Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  Erich O. Richter,et al.  Human Anterior Cingulate Cortex Neurons Encode Cognitive and Emotional Demands , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Functional Imaging of Decision Conflict , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[29]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[30]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  E. Aarts,et al.  Anticipatory activity in anterior cingulate cortex can be independent of conflict and error likelihood. , 2008, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[33]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[34]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[36]  D. Tucker,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses to Errors and Feedback in the Process of Action Regulation , 2003, Psychological science.

[37]  W. Gehring,et al.  Functions of the Medial Frontal Cortex in the Processing of Conflict and Errors , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex integrates reinforcement history to guide voluntary behavior , 2008, Cortex.

[39]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Risk prediction and aversion by anterior cingulate cortex , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[40]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[42]  N. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal regions play a predominant role in imposing an attentional 'set': evidence from fMRI. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[43]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[44]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Attention to simultaneous unrelated auditory and visual events: behavioral and neural correlates. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[46]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[47]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Errors without conflict: Implications for performance monitoring theories of anterior cingulate cortex , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[48]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the GO/NOGO task. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  A M Dale,et al.  Randomized event‐related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI , 1998, Neuroreport.

[51]  Jens Frahm,et al.  Lateralized anterior cingulate function during error processing and conflict monitoring as revealed by high-resolution fMRI. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[53]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex , 2007, NeuroImage.

[54]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Performance Monitoring by the Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Saccade Countermanding , 2003, Science.

[55]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[56]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[57]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[58]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[59]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[60]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[61]  M. Fox,et al.  The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.