Teaching Children with Autism Self‐Initiations as a Pivotal Response

The purpose of this study was to assess whether children with autism could be taught a child-initiated query as a pivotal response to facilitate the use of grammatical morphemes. Data were collected within the context of a multiple baseline design across two children who lacked the use of temporal morphemes. Results of the study indicated that both children learned the self-initiated strategy and both acquired and generalized the targeted morpheme. Additionally, generalized use of the self-initiation into other question forms and concomitant increases in mean length of utterance, verb acquisition, and diversity of verb use occurred for both children. These generalized effects and the applications of this procedure across linguistic targets are discussed.

[1]  P. A. Villiers,et al.  A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech , 1973, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[2]  G. Bartolucci,et al.  Deictic categories in the language of autistic children , 1974, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[3]  D. Scott,et al.  The development of language. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[4]  P. Lightbown,et al.  Structure and variation in child language. , 1975 .

[5]  F. Antinucci,et al.  How children talk about what happened , 1976, Journal of Child Language.

[6]  D. W. Hung Generalization of “curiosity” questioning behavior in autistic children , 1977 .

[7]  R. Jansen,et al.  LANGUAGE ACQUISITION , 1977, The Medical journal of Australia.

[8]  S. Kuczaj The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms , 1977 .

[9]  G. Bartolucci,et al.  Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children , 1980, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[10]  Jon F. Miller,et al.  Assessing Language Production in Children: Experimental Procedures , 1981 .

[11]  L. Harner Children Talk about the Time and Aspect of Actions. , 1981 .

[12]  Children's comprehension of six verb-tense forms. , 1983, Journal of communication disorders.

[13]  A. Wetherby,et al.  Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. , 1984, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[14]  K. Nelson,et al.  Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advances by young children , 1984 .

[15]  M. J. Demetras,et al.  The Expressive Language Characteristics of Autistic Children Compared with Mentally Retarded or Specific Language-Impaired Children , 1985 .

[16]  Paula Menyuk,et al.  Semantic Problems in Autistic Children , 1985 .

[17]  R. Koegel,et al.  Motivation in childhood autism: can they or won't they? , 1985, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[18]  George McCloskey,et al.  Leiter International Performance Scale , 1987 .

[19]  L. Koegel,et al.  A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children , 1987, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  Elizabeth F. Pemberton,et al.  Language facilitation through stories: recasting and modelling , 1987 .

[21]  G. Whitehurst,et al.  The effects of incidental teaching on vocabulary acquisition by young children. , 1988, Child development.

[22]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Books Recieved , 2022, CL.

[23]  J. Sachs,et al.  Cognition and the verb lexicon in early lexical development , 1990, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[24]  D A Behrend,et al.  The development of verb concepts: children's use of verbs to label familiar and novel events. , 1990, Child development.

[25]  G. Conti-Ramsden Maternal recasts and other contingent replies to language-impaired children. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[26]  Rhea Paul,et al.  Communicative initiations in normal and late-talking toddlers , 1991, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[27]  Do children with developmental delays use more frequent and diverse language in verbal routines? , 1992, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[28]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[29]  M. J. Farrar,et al.  Negative Evidence and Grammatical Morpheme Acquisition. , 1992 .

[30]  R. Paul,et al.  Grammatical morpheme acquisition in 4-year-olds with normal, impaired, and late-developing language. , 1993, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[31]  S M Camarata,et al.  Comparison of conversational-recasting and imitative procedures for training grammatical structures in children with specific language impairment. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[32]  P. Hester,et al.  Generalized effects of enhanced Milieu teaching. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[33]  B. A. Taylor,et al.  Teaching children with autism to seek information: acquisition of novel information and generalization of responding. , 1995, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[34]  P. Yoder,et al.  Effect of Verbal Routine Contexts and Expansions on Gains in the Mean Length of Utterance in Children With Developmental Delays , 1995 .

[35]  L. Rescorla,et al.  Normal language acquisition. , 1997, Seminars in pediatric neurology.

[36]  L. Koegel,et al.  Pivotal Response Intervention II: Preliminary Long-Term Outcome Data , 1999 .

[37]  M J Weiss,et al.  Teaching Social Skills to People with Autism , 2001, Behavior modification.

[38]  P. J. Krantz,et al.  Behavior Analysis and Intervention for Adults with Autism , 2002, Behavior modification.

[39]  Dcdre Centner On Relational Meaning : The Acquisition of Verb Meaning , 2003 .