Differential functions of lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) revealed by brain-behavior associations.

We analyzed the behavioral data from 104 neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography or functional magnetic resonance imaging that reported activation peaks in rostral prefrontal cortex (PFC), approximating Brodmann's area 10. The distribution of absolute x coordinates of activation peaks (i.e., x coordinate regardless of hemisphere) differed significantly from a unimodal normal distribution, reflecting distinct clusters of activation in lateral and medial subregions. These 2 clusters were associated with different patterns of behavioral data. Lateral activations were associated with contrasts between experimental and control conditions where response times (RTs) were slower in the experimental condition. Medial activations were associated with contrasts where RTs were, if anything, faster in experimental than control conditions. These findings place important constraints on theories of rostral PFC functions.

[1]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Stephen Lawrie,et al.  Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Frith,et al.  Performance-related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand tasks. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  W. Prinz,et al.  Disorders of volition , 2006 .

[5]  Paul C. Fletcher,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex and the recollection of contextual information , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  P. McLeod,et al.  Measuring the mind speed, control, and age , 2005 .

[7]  Adrian M Owen,et al.  Distinct roles for lateral and medial anterior prefrontal cortex in contextual recollection. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  C. Frith,et al.  Involvement of rostral prefrontal cortex in selection between stimulus‐oriented and stimulus‐independent thought , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  R. Henson What can Functional Neuroimaging Tell the Experimental Psychologist? , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  Jiro Okuda,et al.  Rostral prefrontal brain regions (area 10): A gateway between inner thought and the external world? , 2005 .

[11]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  S. Scott,et al.  The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Perrine Ruby,et al.  A relation between rest and the self in the brain? , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[14]  J. Price,et al.  Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  H J Müller,et al.  Top-down controlled visual dimension weighting: an event-related fMRI study. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[18]  J. Lancaster,et al.  Using the talairach atlas with the MNI template , 2001, NeuroImage.

[19]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[20]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[23]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex , 2000, Psychobiology.

[24]  R. Elliott,et al.  Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[26]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  D. Collins,et al.  Automatic 3D Intersubject Registration of MR Volumetric Data in Standardized Talairach Space , 1994, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[28]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[29]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[30]  D. Pandya,et al.  Supplementary motor area structure and function: Review and hypotheses , 1985 .

[31]  M. Posner Chronometric explorations of mind : the third Paul M. Fitts lectures, delivered at the University of Michigan, September 1976 , 1978 .

[32]  S. Sternberg The discovery of processing stages , 1969 .

[33]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[34]  F. Donders On the speed of mental processes. , 1969, Acta psychologica.