The Impact of Social Roles on Trait Judgments

Consistent with social role theory’s assumption that the role behavior of men and women shapes gender stereotypes, earlier experiments have found that men’s and women’s occupancy of the same role eliminated gender-stereotypical judgments of greater agency and lower communion in men than women. The shifting standards model raises the question of whether a shift to within-sex standards in judgments of men and women in roles could have masked underlying gender stereotypes. To examine this possibility, two experiments obtained judgments of men and women using measures that do or do not restrain shifts to within-sex standards. This measure variation did not affect the social role pattern of smaller perceived sex differences in the presence of role information. These findings thus support the social role theory claim that designations of identical roles for subgroups of men and women eliminate or reduce perceived sex differences.

[1]  T. Eckes,et al.  Social Role Theory of Sex Differences and Similarities : A Current Appraisal , 2012 .

[2]  P. V. Lange,et al.  Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology , 2011 .

[3]  J. A. Smith,et al.  “Having it all” or “doing it all”? Perceived trait attributes and behavioral obligations as a function of workload, parenthood, and gender , 2008 .

[4]  A. Eagly,et al.  Communion and agency judgments of women and men as a function of role information and response format , 2008 .

[5]  Sabine Sczesny,et al.  Die Bedeutung von Informationen zur sozialen Rolle für die Reduktion geschlechtsstereotypen Urteilens , 2007 .

[6]  Amanda B. Diekman,et al.  Rolling with the Changes: A Role Congruity Perspective on Gender Norms , 2006 .

[7]  Donald Tomaskovic-Devey,et al.  Documenting Desegregation: Segregation in American Workplaces by Race, Ethnicity, and Sex, 1966–2003 , 2006 .

[8]  Lisa A. Harrison,et al.  Social Role Theory and the Perceived Gender Role Orientation of Athletes , 2005 .

[9]  Ulrich Kühnen,et al.  Meta-Cognition about Biological Sex and Gender-Stereotypic Physical Appearance: Consequences for the Assessment of Leadership Competence , 2004, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[10]  M. Biernat,et al.  Toward a broader view of social stereotyping. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[11]  Bertram Gawronski Implicational schemata and the correspondence bias: on the diagnostic value of situationally constrained behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  Deborah A. Prentice,et al.  What Women and Men Should Be, Shouldn't be, are Allowed to be, and don't Have to Be: The Contents of Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes , 2002 .

[13]  J. S. Bridges,et al.  Trait Judgments of Stay-At-Home and Employed Parents: A Function of Social Role and/or Shifting Standards? , 2002 .

[14]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Subtyping and Subgrouping: Processes for the Prevention and Promotion of Stereotype Change , 2001 .

[15]  A. Eagly,et al.  Stereotypes as Dynamic Constructs: Women and Men of the Past, Present, and Future , 2000 .

[16]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender-Stereotypic Images of Occupations Correspond to the Sex Segregation of Employment , 1999 .

[17]  C. Crandall,et al.  All that you can be: stereotyping of self and others in a military context. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  M. Biernat,et al.  Decoding Subjective Evaluations: How Stereotypes Provide Shifting Standards , 1997 .

[19]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Equal ratings but separate meanings: Stereotypes and the construal of traits. , 1997 .

[20]  M. Kite Age, Gender, And Occupational Label: A Test of Social Role Theory , 1996 .

[21]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Subtyping versus subgrouping processes in stereotype representation. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  D. Moskowitz,et al.  Situational influences on gender differences in agency and communion. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Stereotypes and the Construal of Individuating Information , 1993 .

[24]  Thomas E. Nelson,et al.  Stereotypes and Standards of Judgment , 1991 .

[25]  A. Eagly,et al.  A Note on Assessing Stereotypes , 1988 .

[26]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender Stereotypes, Occupational Roles, and Beliefs About Part-Time Employees , 1986 .

[27]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles , 1984 .

[28]  D. Buss,et al.  The frequency concept of disposition: dominance and prototypically dominant acts^ , 1980 .

[29]  A. Clifton,et al.  Stereotypes of woman: A single category? , 1976 .

[30]  P. England The Gender Revolution: Uneven and Stalled , 2011 .

[31]  M. Biernat Standards and Expectancies: Contrast and Assimilation in Judgments of Self and Others , 2005 .

[32]  V. E. Schein A Global Look at Psychological Barriers to Women's Progress in Management , 2001 .

[33]  R. Vonk,et al.  Gender subgroups: intergroup bias within the sexes , 1998 .

[34]  M. Biernat The shifting standards model: Implications of stereotype accuracy for social judgment. , 1995 .

[35]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  The correspondence bias. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  M. Biernat,et al.  Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgments. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  A. Eagly Sex differences in social behavior : a social-role interpretation , 1987 .