Extent of compensation for variations in monkey saccadic eye movements

Abstract. We investigated and quantified the ability of the primate saccadic system to generate accurate eye movements in spite of naturally occurring variations in saccadic speed and trajectory. We show that the amplitude of a series of saccades directed to the same target is positively correlated to their peak speed, i.e., the faster the saccade, the bigger its amplitude. We demonstrate that this result cannot be simply accounted for by the main sequence, and that on average the saccadic system is able to compensate for only 61% of the variability in speed. Deviations from the average trajectory are also only partially compensated: the underlying mechanism, which tends to bring the eyes back toward the desired trajectory, underperforms for small movements and overperforms for large movements. We also demonstrate that the performance of this compensatory mechanism, and the metrics of saccades in general, do not depend on the presence of visual information during the movement. By showing that deviations from the desired behavior are corrected during the saccade, our results further support the hypothesis that the innervation signal that generates saccadic eye movements is not pre-programmed but rather is dynamically adjusted during the movement. However, the compensation for deviations from the desired behavior is only partial, and the underlying mechanisms have yet to be completely understood. Although none of the current models of the saccadic system can account for our results, some of them, if appropriately modified, probably could.

[1]  T. Vilis,et al.  The conjugacy of human saccadic eye movements , 1992, Vision Research.

[2]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[3]  Christian Quaia,et al.  Distributed model of control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum , 1998, Neural Networks.

[4]  A. Opstal,et al.  STIMULUS-INDUCED MIDFLIGHT MODIFICATION OF SACCADE TRAJECTORIES , 1987 .

[5]  J. Houk,et al.  Cooperative control of gaze by the superior colliculus, brainstem and cerebellum , 1992 .

[6]  W. Becker,et al.  Human oblique saccades: Quantitative analysis of the relation between horizontal and vertical components , 1990, Vision Research.

[7]  L. Stark,et al.  Time optimal behavior of human saccadic eye movement , 1975 .

[8]  L Stark,et al.  Oblique saccadic eye movements. Independence of horizontal and vertical channels. , 1977, Archives of ophthalmology.

[9]  B. Efron The jackknife, the bootstrap, and other resampling plans , 1987 .

[10]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[11]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  The Initial Direction and Landing Position of Saccades , 1995 .

[12]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .

[13]  E. M. Klier,et al.  Human oculomotor system accounts for 3-D eye orientation in the visual-motor transformation for saccades. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. II. Maps of saccadic deficits. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  N J Gandhi,et al.  Endpoint accuracy in saccades interrupted by stimulation in the omnipause region in monkey , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[16]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of the direction, amplitude, and velocity of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[17]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. I. Curvature of saccadic trajectory. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  L. Optican,et al.  Model of the control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  Initial directions and landing positions of binocular saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[20]  James C. Houk,et al.  A Cerebellar Model of Timing and Prediction in the Control of Reaching , 1999, Neural Computation.

[21]  P. Viviani,et al.  The curvature of oblique saccades , 1977, Vision Research.

[22]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  A parametric analysis of human saccades in different experimental paradigms , 1987, Vision Research.

[23]  D. Guitton,et al.  Brain stem omnipause neurons and the control of combined eye-head gaze saccades in the alert cat. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Modification of saccadic eye movements by GABA-related substances. I. Effect of muscimol and bicuculline in monkey superior colliculus. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Sensorimotor transformation during eye movements to remembered visual targets , 1991, Vision Research.

[26]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Oblique saccadic eye movements of primates. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.

[28]  B Efron,et al.  Statistical Data Analysis in the Computer Age , 1991, Science.

[29]  A. Fuchs,et al.  A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.

[30]  P. Dean,et al.  Modelling the role of the cerebellar fastigial nuclei in producing accurate saccades: the importance of burst timing , 1995, Neuroscience.

[31]  D. Munoz,et al.  Comparison of the discharge characteristics of brain stem omnipause neurons and superior colliculus fixation neurons in monkey: implications for control of fixation and saccade behavior. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  L. Optican,et al.  Slow saccades in spinocerebellar degeneration. , 1976, Archives of neurology.