Are drivers' comparative risk judgments about speeding realistic?

INTRODUCTION This study focused on comparative judgments about speeding risks among young drivers who have a high risk of being involved in a traffic accident. METHOD We examined (a) how these drivers assess their risk of sanctions and their risk of causing an automobile crash because of speeding in comparison to the estimated risks of other drivers, and (b) how realistic their comparative risk judgments are. We measured the relationship between the drivers' comparative risk judgments, self-reported speeding, and driving-related sensation-seeking. We hypothesized that (a) they would think they have less risk of sanctions and of causing a car accident than others, and (b) their comparative judgments of speeding risks would be linked to self-reported speeding and driving-related sensation-seeking. The study was based on a computerized questionnaire survey conducted with 3,002 young drivers (mean age=22.3) administered by professional investigators. RESULTS The results confirmed our hypotheses. IMPACT ON INDUSTRY In order to improve the effectiveness of prevention measures and to evaluate the effect of them, road-safety interventions should take into account comparative risk judgments about the targeted risk behavior.

[1]  F P McKenna,et al.  Exploring the limits of optimism: the case of smokers' decision making. , 1993, British journal of psychology.

[2]  Ian S. Dunn,et al.  Exploring the Limits , 2009 .

[3]  Dongo Rémi Kouabenan,et al.  Perception du risque d'accident lié à l'alcool chez des jeunes automobilistes : quelques déterminants de l'optimisme comparatif , 2004 .

[4]  Ingrid van Schagen,et al.  Driving speed and the risk of road crashes: a review. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[5]  T. Meyer,et al.  Optimisme et pessimisme comparatifs consécutifs àl 'exposition à plusieurs messages menaçants Comparative optimism and pessimism following exposure to several threatening messages , 2007 .

[6]  Marie Helweg-Larsen,et al.  Do Moderators of the Optimistic Bias Affect Personal or Target Risk Estimates? A Review of the Literature , 2001 .

[7]  Anna Sundström,et al.  Self-assessment of driving skill – A review from a measurement perspective , 2008 .

[8]  T. Meyer,et al.  Optimisme et pessimisme comparatifs consécutifs à l'exposition à plusieurs messages menaçants , 2007 .

[9]  B W Bragg,et al.  Perception of the risk of an accident by young and older drivers. , 1986, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[10]  B A Jonah,et al.  Sensation seeking and risky driving: a review and synthesis of the literature. , 1997, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[11]  Lars Åberg,et al.  Speed choice in relation to speed limit and influences from other drivers , 2000 .

[12]  John R. Chambers,et al.  Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  Dana Yagil,et al.  Reasoned Action and Irrational Motives: A Prediction of Drivers' Intention to Violate Traffic Laws1 , 2001 .

[14]  M. Elliott,et al.  Drivers' compliance with speed limits: an application of the theory of planned behavior. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[15]  F. Gibbons,et al.  A longitudinal study of the reciprocal nature of risk behaviors and cognitions in adolescents: what you do shapes what you think, and vice versa. , 1996, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[16]  L Quine,et al.  Perceptions of risk in motorcyclists: unrealistic optimism, relative realism and predictions of behaviour. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[17]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  DEVELOPMENT OF A SENSATION-SEEKING SCALE. , 1964, Journal of consulting psychology.

[18]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  The effect of the commitment to observe speed limits during rehabilitation training courses for traffic regulation offenders in France , 2008 .

[19]  Lars Åberg,et al.  Stability in drivers' speed choice , 2002 .

[20]  L. Perloff,et al.  Self–other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. , 1986 .

[21]  F. McKenna,et al.  Factors underlying illusory self-assessment of driving skill in males and females. , 1991, Accident Analysis and Prevention.

[22]  Talib Rothengatter,et al.  Risk and the absence of pleasure: a motivational approach to modelling road user behaviour , 1988 .

[23]  N. Harré,et al.  Self-enhancement, crash-risk optimism and the impact of safety advertisements on young drivers. , 2005, British journal of psychology.

[24]  P Todesco,et al.  Risk Perception: Unrealistic Optimism or Realistic Expectancy , 1999, Psychological reports.

[25]  A. Guppy,et al.  Subjective probability of accident and apprehension in relation to self-other bias, age, and reported behavior. , 1993, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  Peter R. Harris,et al.  The impact of perceived experience on likelihood judgments for self and others: an experimental approach , 2007 .

[27]  P. Delhomme,et al.  Quand chacun pense être moins exposé que les autres aux risques mais plus réceptif aux messages de prévention pour la santé. , 2000 .

[28]  Patricia Delhomme Evaluation d'actions possibles face a un risque : une approche experimentale de l'effet du controle subjectif sur l'optimisme absolu et comparatif , 2001 .

[29]  W M Klein Objective standards are not enough: affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Dongo Rémi Kouabenan,et al.  Jugements comparatifs et absolus de deux risques routiers contexualisés et raisons invoquées quant à ces jugements , 2005 .

[31]  Patricia Delhomme Optimisme comparatif chez les usagers de la route : Une protection contre le risque ? , 2000 .

[32]  Allan F Williams,et al.  Characteristics of speeders. , 2006, Journal of safety research.

[33]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Situated Optimism: Specific Outcome Expectancies and Self-Regulation , 1998 .

[34]  L. Åberg,et al.  OBSERVED VEHICLE SPEED AND DRIVERS' PERCEIVED SPEED OF OTHERS , 1997 .

[35]  W. Klein,et al.  Objective standards are not enough: affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  Patricia Delhomme,et al.  Risk comparative judgments while driving a car among competitive road cyclists and non-cyclists , 2009 .

[37]  T. Lajunen,et al.  Can we trust self-reports of driving? Effects of impression management on driver behaviour questionnaire responses , 2003 .

[38]  Lynda Chin,et al.  Guest Editor , 2003, Oncogene.

[39]  C W Dent,et al.  Perceived invulnerability and cigarette smoking among adolescents. , 2000, Addictive behaviors.

[40]  P. Delhomme,et al.  Comparer sa vitesse a celle d'autrui. Comparaison sociale et representation de la conduite chez les automobilistes europeens , 2000 .

[41]  M L Matthews,et al.  Age differences in male drivers' perception of accident risk: the role of perceived driving ability. , 1986, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[42]  Bernard Guerin,et al.  What Do People Think About the Risks of Driving? Implications for Traffic Safety Interventions1 , 1994 .

[43]  P Delhomme LIENS ENTRE LA SURESTIMATION DE SES PROPRES CAPACITES, L'EXPERIENCE DE LA CONDUITE ET L'ACTIVITE DE LA CONDUITE , 1994 .

[44]  P Delhomme,et al.  Reducing air-pollution: a new argument for getting drivers to abide by the speed limit? , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[45]  P. Harris,et al.  The illusion of control and optimism about health: on being less at risk but no more in control than others. , 1994, The British journal of social psychology.