Public Praise vs. Private Pay: Effects of Rewards on Energy Conservation in the Workplace

Any solution to rising levels of CO2 depends on human behavior. One common approach to changing human behavior is rewarding desired behavior. Because financial incentives often have side effects that diminish efficacy, we predict that more psychologically oriented social rewards are more effective, because they invoke adherence to descriptive and injunctive social norms. We investigated this by measuring electricity use for 13 weeks at a Dutch firm. Each week, employees were rewarded for conserving energy. They either received monetary rewards (€0-€5) or social rewards (grade points with a descriptive comment). Rewards were either private or public. In both the short and long term, public rewards outperformed private rewards, and social rewards outperformed monetary rewards. This suggests that private monetary rewards, although popular, may be ineffective. Instead, public social rewards may be a more promising approach to stimulating energy conservation. Such social rewards do not crowd out intrinsic motivation, have less need for large-scale institutions or exogenous funding, and work regardless of who is paying the energy bill. Thus, we argue that the social norms approach should be considered more frequently as a valuable tool in the intervention tool-kit, especially when focusing on low-cost environmental behavior.

[1]  Noah J. Goldstein,et al.  Normative Social Influence is Underdetected , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[2]  Samuel Bowles,et al.  Policies Designed for Self-Interested Citizens May Undermine "The Moral Sentiments": Evidence from Economic Experiments , 2008, Science.

[3]  Siegwart Lindenberg Intrinsic motivation in a new light , 2001 .

[4]  B. Frey,et al.  Motivation Crowding Theory: A Survey of Empirical Evidence , 2000 .

[5]  Jesse Graham,et al.  Conserving Energy by Inducing People to Drive Less , 2011 .

[6]  R. D. Young,et al.  Changing Behavior and Making it Stick The Conceptualization and Management of Conservation Behavior , 1993 .

[7]  Robert B. Cialdini,et al.  The transsituational influence of social norms. , 1993 .

[8]  E. Fehr,et al.  Psychological Foundations of Incentives , 2002 .

[9]  Lowell W. Busenitz,et al.  Differences between entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: Biases and heuristics in strategic decision-making , 1997 .

[10]  D. Kahneman Maps of Bounded Rationality: Psychology for Behavioral Economics , 2003 .

[11]  E. Deci,et al.  A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  M. Dekay,et al.  Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Nina Mazar,et al.  Large stakes and big mistakes , 2009 .

[14]  T. J. Gerpott,et al.  Determinants of willingness to pay for smart meters: An empirical analysis of household customers in Germany , 2013 .

[15]  H. Wilke,et al.  Undermining trust and cooperation : The paradox of sanctioning systems in social dilemmas , 2006 .

[16]  S. Barr,et al.  The household energy gap: examining the divide between habitual- and purchase-related conservation behaviours , 2005 .

[17]  P. Stern Information, Incentives, and Proenvironmental Consumer Behavior , 1999 .

[18]  Shanjun Li,et al.  Gasoline Prices, Government Support, and the Demand for Hybrid Vehicles in the U.S. , 2009 .

[19]  C. Vlek,et al.  A review of intervention studies aimed at household energy conservation , 2005 .

[20]  S Pacala,et al.  Stabilization Wedges: Solving the Climate Problem for the Next 50 Years with Current Technologies , 2004, Science.

[21]  G. T. Gardner,et al.  The Short List: The Most Effective Actions U.S. Households Can Take to Curb Climate Change , 2008 .

[22]  Robert J. Waller,et al.  Social Traps , 1980, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[23]  L. Festinger A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .

[24]  L. Steg,et al.  Normative, Gain and Hedonic Goal Frames Guiding Environmental Behavior , 2007 .

[25]  P. Kollock SOCIAL DILEMMAS: The Anatomy of Cooperation , 1998 .

[26]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  The norm of self-interest. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[27]  S. Mullainathan,et al.  Behavior and Energy Policy , 2010, Science.

[28]  P. Stern,et al.  Psychological research for the new energy problems: Strategies and opportunities. , 1992 .

[29]  N. Kerr Motivation losses in small groups: a social dilemma analysis , 1983 .

[30]  Richard P. Larrick,et al.  The MPG Illusion , 2008, Science.

[31]  P. Wesley Schultz,et al.  Changing Behavior With Normative Feedback Interventions: A Field Experiment on Curbside Recycling , 1999 .

[32]  Shanjun Li,et al.  Gasoline Prices, Government Support, and the Demand for Hybrid Vehicles in the United States , 2011 .

[33]  Fred Luthans,et al.  The impact of financial and nonfinancial incentives on business-unit outcomes over time. , 2006, The Journal of applied psychology.

[34]  Charles D. Samuelson Energy conservation : a social dilemma approach , 1990 .

[35]  Noah J. Goldstein,et al.  The Constructive, Destructive, and Reconstructive Power of Social Norms , 2007, Psychological science.

[36]  A. Bandura Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[37]  G. T. Gardner,et al.  Household actions can provide a behavioral wedge to rapidly reduce US carbon emissions , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  E. Scott Geller,et al.  Behavior Analysis and Environmental Protection: Accomplishments and Potential for More , 2004 .

[39]  P. Lockwood,et al.  Emphasizing Jobs and Trees: Increasing the Impact of Proenvironmental Messages on Migrants , 2011 .