Functional evidence for cone‐specific connectivity in the human retina

Physiological studies of colour vision have not yet resolved the controversial issue of how chromatic opponency is constructed at a neuronal level. Two competing theories, the cone‐selective hypothesis and the random‐wiring hypothesis, are currently equivocal to the architecture of the primate retina. In central vision, both schemes are capable of producing colour opponency due to the fact that receptive field centres receive input from a single bipolar cell – the so called ‘private line arrangement’. However, in peripheral vision this single‐cone input to the receptive field centre is lost, so that any random cone connectivity would result in a predictable reduction in the quality of colour vision. Behavioural studies thus far have indeed suggested a selective loss of chromatic sensitivity in peripheral vision. We investigated chromatic sensitivity as a function of eccentricity for the cardinal chromatic (L/M and S/(L + M)) and achromatic (L + M) pathways, adopting stimulus size as the critical variable. Results show that performance can be equated across the visual field simply by a change of scale (size). In other words, there exists no qualitative loss of chromatic sensitivity across the visual field. Critically, however, the quantitative nature of size dependency for each of the cardinal chromatic and achromatic mechanisms is very specific, reinforcing their independence in terms of anatomy and genetics. Our data provide clear evidence for a physiological model of primate colour vision that retains chromatic quality in peripheral vision, thus supporting the cone‐selective hypothesis.

[1]  David Whitaker,et al.  The influence of eccentricity on position and movement acuities as revealed by spatial scaling , 1992, Vision Research.

[2]  B. Boycott,et al.  Functional architecture of the mammalian retina. , 1991, Physiological reviews.

[3]  R. L. Valois,et al.  Analysis of response patterns of LGN cells. , 1966, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[4]  Barry B. Lee,et al.  Center surround receptive field structure of cone bipolar cells in primate retina , 2000, Vision Research.

[5]  R. Shapley,et al.  Spatial structure of cone inputs to receptive fields in primate lateral geniculate nucleus , 1992, Nature.

[6]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in striate cortex of macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  S. Klein,et al.  Detection and discrimination of the direction of motion in central and peripheral vision of normal and amblyopic observers , 1984, Vision Research.

[8]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  Barry B. Lee,et al.  The 'blue-on' opponent pathway in primate retina originates from a distinct bistratified ganglion cell type , 1994, Nature.

[10]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Sensitivity to spatiotemporal colour contrast in the peripheral visual field , 1983, Vision Research.

[11]  R. Shapley,et al.  Space and Time Maps of Cone Photoreceptor Signals in Macaque Lateral Geniculate Nucleus , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  The Design of Chromatically Opponent Receptive Fields , 1991 .

[13]  I. Abramov,et al.  Color appearance in the peripheral retina: effects of stimulus size. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[14]  David J. Calkins,et al.  M and L cones in macaque fovea connect to midget ganglion cells by different numbers of excitatory synapses , 1994, Nature.

[15]  K. Mullen The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red‐green and blue‐yellow chromatic gratings. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  D. Dacey Parallel pathways for spectral coding in primate retina. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[17]  R. Shapley,et al.  Cat and monkey retinal ganglion cells and their visual functional roles , 1986, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  R. Shapley,et al.  The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  D. Dacey Primate retina: cell types, circuits and color opponency , 1999, Progress in Retinal and Eye Research.

[20]  S. Karplus,et al.  ‘Foveal Tritanopia’ , 1947, Nature.

[21]  K. Mullen,et al.  Losses in Peripheral Colour Sensitivity Predicted from “Hit and Miss” Post-receptoral Cone Connections , 1996, Vision Research.

[22]  K. Mullen,et al.  Colour vision as a post-receptoral specialization of the central visual field , 1991, Vision Research.

[23]  N. Drasdo The neural representation of visual space , 1977, Nature.

[24]  A. Milam,et al.  Distribution and morphology of human cone photoreceptors stained with anti‐blue opsin , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[25]  Angela M. Brown Color Vision: From Genes to Perception. , 2001 .

[26]  A. Johnston,et al.  Spatial scaling of central and peripheral contrast-sensitivity functions. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[27]  J. Rovamo,et al.  Cortical magnification factor predicts the photopic contrast sensitivity of peripheral vision , 1978, Nature.

[28]  A. Cowey,et al.  Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  David J. Calkins,et al.  Microcircuitry and Mosaic of a Blue–Yellow Ganglion Cell in the Primate Retina , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Dacey The mosaic of midget ganglion cells in the human retina , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  R Näsänen,et al.  Cortical magnification and peripheral vision. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[32]  Paul R. Martin,et al.  Chromatic sensitivity of ganglion cells in the peripheral primate retina , 2001, Nature.

[33]  Jyrki Rovamo,et al.  Modelling the dependence of contrast sensitivity on grating area and spatial frequency , 1993, Vision Research.

[34]  D H Kelly,et al.  Opponent-color receptive-field profiles determined from large-area psychophysical measurements. , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[35]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Cardinal directions of color space , 1982, Vision Research.

[36]  J. Moreland,et al.  Colour Perception with the Peripheral Retina , 1959 .

[37]  A B Watson,et al.  Estimation of local spatial scale. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[38]  R. Hess,et al.  Human peripheral spatial resolution for achromatic and chromatic stimuli: limits imposed by optical and retinal factors. , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  Rhea T Eskew,et al.  Chromatic detection and discrimination in the periphery: a postreceptoral loss of color sensitivity. , 2003, Visual neuroscience.

[40]  B. Boycott,et al.  Cortical magnification factor and the ganglion cell density of the primate retina , 1989, Nature.

[41]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Spatial Structure of Cone Inputs to Color Cells in Alert Macaque Primary Visual Cortex (V-1) , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  D. Dacey,et al.  Dendritic field size and morphology of midget and parasol ganglion cells of the human retina. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  D. Hubel,et al.  Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. , 1966, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  D. Dacey,et al.  This paper was presented at a colloquium entitled ‘ ‘ Vision : From Photon to Perception , ’ ’ organized by , 1998 .

[45]  J. Nathans,et al.  Molecular genetics of human color vision: the genes encoding blue, green, and red pigments. , 1986, Science.

[46]  I Abramov,et al.  Color vision in the peripheral retina. II. Hue and saturation. , 1977, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[47]  R. L. Valois,et al.  Some transformations of color information from lateral geniculate nucleus to striate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[49]  E. Callaway,et al.  Parallel colour-opponent pathways to primary visual cortex , 2003, Nature.

[50]  David R. Williams,et al.  The design of chromatically opponent receptive fields , 1991 .

[51]  J. Mollon Color vision. , 1982, Annual review of psychology.

[52]  W. Paulus,et al.  A new concept of retinal colour coding , 1983, Vision Research.

[53]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  A multi-stage color model. , 1993, Vision research.

[54]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  How big is a Gabor patch, and why should we care? , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[55]  K. Mullen,et al.  Differential distributions of red–green and blue–yellow cone opponency across the visual field , 2002, Visual Neuroscience.

[56]  Angela M. Brown,et al.  Higher order color mechanisms , 1986, Vision Research.

[57]  B. B. Lee,et al.  Receptive fields of primate retinal ganglion cells studied with a novel technique , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[58]  Leo Maurice Hurvich,et al.  Color vision , 1981 .

[59]  Mary A. Johnson,et al.  Color vision in the peripheral retina. , 1986 .