On the Primacy of Behavioral Research for Understanding the Brain

Understanding the brain requires us to answer both what the brain does, and how it does it. Using a series of examples, I make the case that behavior is often more useful than neuroscientific measurements for answering the first question. Moreover, I show that even for “how” questions that pertain to neural mechanism, a well-crafted behavioral paradigm can offer deeper insight and stronger constraints on computational and mechanistic models than do many highly challenging (and very expensive) neural studies. I conclude that purely behavioral research is essential for understanding the brain—especially its cognitive functions—contrary to the opinion of prominent funding bodies and some scientific journals, who erroneously place neural data on a pedestal and consider behavior to be subsidiary.

[1]  Y. Niv,et al.  Model-based decision making and model-free learning , 2020, Current Biology.

[2]  Steve W. C. Chang,et al.  Is There a ‘Social’ Brain? Implementations and Algorithms , 2020, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  S. Leslie,et al.  Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science , 2020 .

[4]  Kenneth A. Norman,et al.  Reward prediction errors create event boundaries in memory , 2019, Cognition.

[5]  Darius E. Parvin,et al.  Neural Signatures of Prediction Errors in a Decision-Making Task Are Modulated by Action Execution Failures , 2018, Current Biology.

[6]  N. Daw Are we of two minds? , 2018, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  M. Pinsk,et al.  A Dynamic Interplay within the Frontoparietal Network Underlies Rhythmic Spatial Attention , 2018, Neuron.

[8]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Independent Attention Mechanisms Control the Activation of Tactile and Visual Working Memory Representations , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Y. Niv,et al.  Dissociable effects of surprising rewards on learning and memory , 2017, bioRxiv.

[10]  Yael Niv,et al.  Reinforcement learning with Marr , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[11]  Matthew J. Crossley,et al.  Credit assignment in movement-dependent reinforcement learning , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  Contextual effects in visual working memory reveal hierarchically structured memory representations. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[13]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Reinforcement Learning in Multidimensional Environments Relies on Attention Mechanisms , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Roiser What has neuroscience ever done for us , 2015 .

[15]  T. Griffiths Manifesto for a new (computational) cognitive revolution , 2015, Cognition.

[16]  P. Dayan,et al.  Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits , 2014, Molecular Psychiatry.

[17]  S. Corkin,et al.  H.M.'s contributions to neuroscience: A review and autopsy studies , 2014, Hippocampus.

[18]  W. Ma,et al.  Changing concepts of working memory , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Y. Saalmann,et al.  Rhythmic Sampling within and between Objects despite Sustained Attention at a Cued Location , 2013, Current Biology.

[20]  E. Marder,et al.  From the connectome to brain function , 2013, Nature Methods.

[21]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  No Evidence for an Item Limit in Change Detection , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[22]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Variability in encoding precision accounts for visual short-term memory limitations , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  P. Dayan,et al.  Neural Prediction Errors Reveal a Risk-Sensitive Reinforcement-Learning Process in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  D. Lloyd Is "Cognitive Neuroscience" an Oxymoron? , 2012 .

[25]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[26]  R. Saxe,et al.  Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Invariant Object Identification A Neural Network Model of , 2010 .

[28]  Y. Niv Reinforcement learning in the brain , 2009 .

[29]  Xiao-Jing Wang Decision Making in Recurrent Neuronal Circuits , 2008, Neuron.

[30]  Paul M Bays,et al.  Dynamic Shifts of Limited Working Memory Resources in Human Vision , 2008, Science.

[31]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  Ehren L. Newman,et al.  A neural network model of retrieval-induced forgetting. , 2007, Psychological review.

[33]  Rachel A Robbins,et al.  No face-like processing for objects-of-expertise in three behavioural tasks , 2007, Cognition.

[34]  M. Coltheart What has Functional Neuroimaging told us about the Mind (so far)? (Position Paper Presented to the European Cognitive Neuropsychology Workshop, Bressanone, 2005) , 2006, Cortex.

[35]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[37]  Kenneth A. Norman,et al.  How Inhibitory Oscillations Can Train Neural Networks and Punish Competitors , 2006, Neural Computation.

[38]  Claus-Christian Carbon,et al.  Face-specific configural processing of relational information. , 2006, British journal of psychology.

[39]  B. Balleine Neural bases of food-seeking: Affect, arousal and reward in corticostriatolimbic circuits , 2005, Physiology & Behavior.

[40]  P. Dayan,et al.  Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  Brenda Milner,et al.  The medial temporal-lobe amnesic syndrome. , 2005, The Psychiatric clinics of North America.

[42]  B. Balleine,et al.  Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action–outcome learning in instrumental conditioning , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[43]  C. Koch,et al.  Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain , 2005, Nature.

[44]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Face perception: domain specific, not process specific. , 2004, Neuron.

[45]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  B. Balleine,et al.  Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[48]  W. Schultz,et al.  Coding of Predicted Reward Omission by Dopamine Neurons in a Conditioned Inhibition Paradigm , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Michael C. Anderson Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of forgetting. , 2003 .

[50]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[51]  S. Killcross,et al.  Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  S. Hochstein,et al.  View from the Top Hierarchies and Reverse Hierarchies in the Visual System , 2002, Neuron.

[53]  B. Balleine,et al.  The Role of Learning in the Operation of Motivational Systems , 2002 .

[54]  W. Schultz,et al.  Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory , 2001, Nature.

[55]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[56]  J. Gray,et al.  Impaired associative learning in chronic schizophrenics and their first-degree relatives: A study of latent inhibition and the Kamin blocking effect , 2001, Schizophrenia Research.

[57]  T. Zentall,et al.  “work ethic” in pigeons: Reward value is directly related to the effort or time required to obtain the reward , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[58]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[59]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning , 1997, Nature.

[61]  P. Dayan,et al.  A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[62]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[63]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Bee foraging in uncertain environments using predictive hebbian learning , 1995, Nature.

[64]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: memory retrieval as a model case. , 1995, Psychological review.

[65]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[66]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Attentional control of early perceptual learning. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[67]  W. Schultz,et al.  Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[68]  M. Gabriel,et al.  Learning and Computational Neuroscience: Foundations of Adaptive Networks , 1990 .

[69]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Time-Derivative Models of Pavlovian Reinforcement , 1990 .

[70]  R. Rescorla Pavlovian conditioning. It's not what you think it is. , 1988, The American psychologist.

[71]  D. Baylor,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[72]  S. Carey,et al.  Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[73]  A. Dickinson Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy , 1985 .

[74]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Neuronlike adaptive elements that can solve difficult learning control problems , 1983, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[75]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[76]  R. Yin Looking at Upside-down Faces , 1969 .

[77]  S. Ochs Integrative Activity of the Brain: An Interdisciplinary Approach , 1968 .

[78]  L. Kamin Attention-like processes in classical conditioning , 1967 .

[79]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group. , 1959 .

[80]  W. Stiles COLOR VISION: THE APPROACH THROUGH INCREMENT-THRESHOLD SENSITIVITY. , 1959 .

[81]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[82]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[83]  A. Hodgkin,et al.  A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve , 1952, The Journal of physiology.