Improving individuals' and groups' abilities to solve problems and make decisions is recognized as an important issue in education, industry, and government. Recent research has identified a prescriptive model of problem solving, although there is less agreement as to appropriate techniques. Separate research on personality and cognitive styles has identified important individual differences in how people approach and solve problems and make decisions. This paper relates a model of the problem-solving process to Jung's theory of personality types (as measured by the MBTI) and identifies specific techniques to support individual differences. The recent transition to the information age has focused attention on the processes of problem solving and decision making and their improvement (e.g., Nickerson, Perkins, & Smith, 1985; Stice, 1987; Whimbey & Lochhead, 1982). In fact, Gagne (1974, 1984) considers the strategies used in these processes to be a primary outcome of modern education. Although there is increasing agreement regarding the prescriptive steps to be used in problem solving, there is less consensus on specific techniques to be employed at each step in the problem-solving/decisionmaking process. There is concurrent and parallel research on personality and cognitive styles that describes individuals' preferred patterns for approaching problems and decisions and their utilization of specific skills required by these processes (e.g., encoding, storage, retrieval, etc.). Researchers have studied the relationship between personality characteristics and problem-solving strategies (e.g., Heppner, Neal, & Larson, 1984; Hopper & Kirschenbaum, 1985; Myers, 1980), with Jung's (1971) theory on psychological type serving as the basis for much of this work, especially as measured by the MBTI (Myers & McCaulley, 1985). One conclusion that may be drawn from these investigations is that individual differences in problem solving and decision making must be considered to adequately understand the dynamics of these processes (Stice, 1987). Attention must be paid to both the problem-solving process and the specific techniques associated with important personal characteristics. That is, individuals and organizations must have a problem-solving process as well as specific techniques congruent with individual styles if they are to capitalize on these areas of current research. McCaulley (1987) attempted to do this by first focusing on individual differences in personality and then by presenting four steps for problem solving based on Jung's (1971) four mental processes (sensing, intuition, thinking, and feeling). Another strategy would be to consider first the problem-solving process and then to integrate individual preferences or patterns within this process. This second strategy is the perspective of this paper. The purpose of this paper is to relate a model of the problem-solving process to a theory of personality type and temperaments in order to facilitate problem solving by focusing on important individual differences. Specific techniques that can be used in the problemsolving/decision-making process to take advantage of these differences are also identified. The integrated process is applicable to a variety of individual and group situations. Problem-Solving and Decision-Making Process Problem solving is a process in which we perceive and resolve a gap between a present situation and a desired goal, with the path to the goal blocked by known or unknown obstacles. In general, the situation is one not previously encountered, or where at least a specific solution from past experiences is not known. In contrast, decision making is a selection process where one of two or more possible solutions is chosen to reach a desired goal. The steps in both problem solving and decision making are quite similar. In fact, the terms are sometimes used interchangeably. Most models of problem solving and decision making include at least four phases (e.g., Bransford & Stein, 1984; Dewey, 1933; Polya, 1971): 1) an Input phase in which a problem is perceived and an attempt is made to understand the situation or problem; 2) a Processing phase in which alternatives are generated and evaluated and a solution is selected; 3) an Output phase which includes planning for and implementing the solution; and 4) a Review phase in which the solution is evaluated and modifications are made, if necessary. Most researchers describe the problem-solving/decision-making process as beginning with the perception of a gap and ending with the implementation and evaluation of a solution to fill that gap. Each phase of the process includes specific steps to be completed before moving to the next phase. These steps will be discussed in greater detail later in this paper. Consideration of Individual Differences Although there are a variety of ways to consider individual differences relative to problem solving and decision making, this paper will focus on personality type and temperament as measured by the MBTI. Personality Type and Problem Solving Researchers have investigated the relationship of Jung's theory of individuals' preferences and their approach to problem solving and decision making (e.g., Lawrence, 1982, 1984; McCaulley, 1987; Myers & McCaulley, 1985). The following is a summary of their findings. When solving problems, individuals preferring introversion will want to take time to think and clarify their ideas before they begin talking, while those preferring extraversion will want to talk through their ideas in order to clarify them. In addition, Is will more likely be concerned with their own understanding of important concepts and ideas, while Es will continually seek feedback from the environment about the viability of their ideas. Sensing individuals will be more likely to pay attention to facts, details, and reality. They will also tend to select standard solutions that have worked in the past. Persons with intuition preferences, on the other hand, will more likely attend to the meaningfulness of the facts, the relationships among the facts, and the possibilities of future events that can be imagined from these facts. They will exhibit a tendency to develop new, original solutions rather than to use what has worked previously. Individuals with a thinking preference will tend to use logic and analysis during problem solving. They are also likely to value objectivity and to be impersonal in drawing conclusions. They will want solutions to make sense in terms of the facts, models, and/or principles under consideration. By contrast, individuals with a feeling preference are more likely to consider values and feelings in the problem-solving process. They will tend to be subjective in their decision making and to consider how their decisions could affect other people. The final dimension to be considered describes an individual's preference for either judging (using T or F) or perceiving (using S or N). Js are more likely to prefer structure and organization and will want the problem-solving process to demonstrate closure. Ps are more likely to prefer flexibility and adaptability. They will be more concerned that the problemsolving process considers a variety of techniques and provides for unforeseen change. As a demonstration of how personality type can affect problem solving, McCaulley (1987) describes the problem-solving characteristics of two of the 16 MBTI types, ISTJ and ENFP. In problem solving, ISTJ will want a clear idea of the problem (I) and attack it by looking for the facts (S) and by relying on a logical, impersonal (T), step-by-step approach in reaching conclusions. In contrast, ENFP will throw out all sorts of possibilities (N), seeking feedback from the environment to clarify the problem (E). Brainstorming (NP) will be enjoyed. The human aspects of the problem (F) are likely to be emphasized over impersonal, technical issues (T). To the ISTJ, the ENFP approach is likely to seem irrational or scattered. To the ENFP, the ISTJ approach is likely to seem slow and unimaginative. (pp. 43-44)
[1]
Donald R. Woods,et al.
How might i teach problem solving
,
1987
.
[2]
J. Stanley,et al.
Book Review: Taxonomy of Educational Objectives, The Classification of Educational Goals, Handbook I: Cognitive Domain
,
1957
.
[3]
M. Lakin.
Interpersonal encounter: theory and practice in sensitivity training
,
1971
.
[4]
L. A. Hausman.
How we Think
,
1921
.
[5]
Norman Frederiksen,et al.
IMPLICATIONS OF COGNITIVE THEORY FOR INSTRUCTION IN PROBLEM SOLVING
,
1983
.
[6]
C. Bereiter,et al.
From behaviourism to cognitive behaviourism to cognitive development: Steps in the evolution of instructional design
,
1984
.
[7]
Khateeb M. Hussain,et al.
Network analysis for educational management
,
1969
.
[8]
Jack R. Fraenkel.
How to teach about values : an analytic approach / Jack R. Fraenkel
,
1977
.
[9]
R. Feuerstein.
Instrumental Enrichment: An Intervention Program for Cognitive Modifiability
,
1980
.
[10]
Edward De Bono,et al.
De Bono's Thinking Course
,
1982
.
[11]
Arthur Whimbey,et al.
Problem solving and comprehension
,
1982
.
[12]
E. Marcus.
Developing Critical Thinkers: Challenging Adults to Explore Alternative Ways of Thinking and Acting
,
1988
.
[13]
G. W. Neal,et al.
Problem‐Solving Training as Prevention with College Students
,
1984
.
[14]
R. Gagne.
Learning outcomes and their effects: Useful categories of human performance.
,
1984
.
[15]
Teaching Study Skills: A Guide for Teachers
,
1981
.
[16]
M. Mccaulley,et al.
The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator: A Jungian Model for Problem Solving.
,
1987
.
[17]
I. B. Myers.
Manual: A Guide to the Development and Use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator
,
1985
.
[18]
Allen Newell,et al.
Human Problem Solving.
,
1973
.
[19]
Michael K. Gardner,et al.
Cognitive Psychological Approaches to Instructional Task Analysis
,
1985
.
[20]
Jacquelyn Wonder,et al.
Whole-Brain Thinking: Working from Both Sides of the Brain to Achieve Peak Job Performance
,
1984
.
[21]
R. Mayer,et al.
Hidden costs of reflectiveness: aspects of successful scientific reasoning
,
1988
.
[22]
Robert H. McKim,et al.
Thinking Visually: A Strategy Manual for Problem Solving
,
1980
.
[23]
David W. Johnson,et al.
Critical Thinking through Structured Controversy.
,
1988
.
[24]
Moshe F. Rubinstein,et al.
Tools for thinking and problem solving
,
1985
.
[25]
R. Feuerstein,et al.
The dynamic assessment of retarded performers
,
1979
.
[26]
Benjamin S. Bloom,et al.
Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: The Classification of Educational Goals.
,
1957
.
[27]
Robert M. Gagné,et al.
Essentials of Learning for Instruction
,
1988
.
[28]
R. Sternberg.
The Triarchic Mind: A New Theory of Human Intelligence
,
1988
.
[29]
I. Janis,et al.
Decision Making: A Psychological Analysis of Conflict, Choice, and Commitment
,
1977
.
[30]
D. Kolb.
Experiential Learning: Experience as the Source of Learning and Development
,
1983
.
[31]
D. Keirsey,et al.
Please Understand Me: Character and Temperament Types
,
1978
.
[32]
Howard Kirschenbaum.
Advanced Value Clarification
,
1977
.
[33]
J. Bransford,et al.
The ideal problem solver
,
1984
.
[34]
Herbert A. Simon,et al.
The Sciences of the Artificial
,
1970
.
[35]
David C. Berliner,et al.
Review of research in education
,
1973
.