Seeing is believing: Utilization of subliminal symbols requires a visible relevant context

Sensory input that is not available for conscious report can still affect our behavior. Recent findings suggest that such subliminal information has the potency to influence behavior in a way that is dependent on the observer’s current intentions. Here, we investigate whether conscious observation of stimulus relevance provides an incentive for the utilization of nonconscious stimuli. We manipulated the predictive power of directional cues to selectively affect the incentive to utilize them for a subsequent target detection task. Central arrow cues rendered invisible by interocular suppression elicited a facilitatory cuing effect, but only when intermixed with visible arrow cues that were highly predictive with respect to (i.e., 80 % congruent with) the subsequent target location. When the visible cues were nonpredictive (50 % congruent), no subliminal cuing effect was found. An analysis of learning effects corroborates these findings; Cuing effects elicited by both visible and invisible cues increased over the course of the experiment, but only when intermixed visible cues were highly predictive. In a second experiment, we demonstrated that the intrinsic relevance of invisible cues (either 50 % or 100 % congruent) has no effect on the utilization of visible cues. We conclude that conscious perception is required to make statistical inferences about the relevance of symbolic cues. Once statistical information is extracted consciously, it affects subsequent nonconscious processing in a way that fits the current context. Accordingly, one of the possible functions of consciousness could be to extract general rules out of the conscious information, to provide guidelines for future behavior.

[1]  U. Mattler,et al.  Masked stimuli modulate endogenous shifts of spatial attention , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[2]  Brian N. Pasley,et al.  Subcortical Discrimination of Unperceived Objects during Binocular Rivalry , 2004, Neuron.

[3]  Fang Fang,et al.  A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Event perception and the word repetition effect , 1988 .

[5]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Mechanisms of subliminal response priming , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[6]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Suppressed images selectively affect the dominant percept during binocular rivalry. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[7]  R. Blake,et al.  Neural bases of binocular rivalry , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Unconscious Automatic Processes: Evidence from Event-related Potentials in a Masked Priming Paradigm , 2006 .

[10]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[12]  Victor A. F. Lamme Blindsight: the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[13]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[14]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  S. Kouider,et al.  Longer is not better: nonconscious overstimulation reverses priming influences under interocular suppression , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[17]  R. Verleger,et al.  How the Self Controls Its Automatic Pilot when Processing Subliminal Information , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Horstmann,et al.  Author ' s personal copy Attentional capture by masked colour singletons , 2010 .

[19]  Sheng He,et al.  Cortical Responses to Invisible Faces: Dissociating Subsystems for Facial-Information Processing , 2006, Current Biology.

[20]  L. Zhaoping Attention capture by eye of origin singletons even without awareness--a hallmark of a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[21]  K. Forster,et al.  Unconscious cognition isn’t that smart: Modulation of masked repetition priming effect in the word naming task , 2008, Cognition.

[22]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic Control of Visual Attention , 2001, Psychological science.

[23]  Lauri Nummenmaa,et al.  Visuospatial attention shifts by gaze and arrow cues: An ERP study , 2008, Brain Research.

[24]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and performance IX , 1982 .

[25]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  P. Roelfsema,et al.  A monocular, unconscious form of visual attention. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[27]  R. Blake A Neural Theory of Binocular Rivalry , 1989 .

[28]  Sheng He,et al.  Seeing the invisible: The scope and limits of unconscious processing in binocular rivalry , 2008 .

[29]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Local field potential reflects perceptual suppression in monkey visual cortex , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[31]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  M. Damian,et al.  Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: automaticity rather than semantic processing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activation , 2000, Neuroreport.

[34]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Multiple bottlenecks in information processing? An electrophysiological examination , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  M. Aravindh Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2010 .

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.

[37]  Effective processing of masked eye gaze requires volitional control , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Lauri Nummenmaa,et al.  Gaze distractors influence saccadic curvature: Evidence for the role of the oculomotor system in gaze-cued orienting , 2006, Vision Research.

[39]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[41]  F. Fang,et al.  Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  Frank Tong,et al.  Competing Theories of Binocular Rivalry: A Possible Resolution , 2001 .

[43]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition , 2003, Cognition.

[44]  O. Neumann,et al.  Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cuing task. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  U. Ansorge,et al.  Shifts of visuospatial attention to invisible (metacontrast-masked) singletons: Clues from reaction times and event-related potential , 2006 .

[47]  B. Breitmeyer Visual masking: past accomplishments, present status, future developments , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[48]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Inhibition of return in response to gaze cues: The roles of time course and fixation cue , 2007 .

[49]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  Len Thomas,et al.  Retrospective Power Analysis , 1997 .

[51]  R. Blake © 2001 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 5 A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies , 2000 .

[52]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[53]  J. Tipples Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[54]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  Gender aftereffects in the perception of silhouetted face profiles , 2010 .

[56]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Implicit Perceptual Anticipation Triggered by Statistical Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[58]  P. Sterzer,et al.  A direct oculomotor correlate of unconscious visual processing , 2012, Current Biology.

[59]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[61]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness: The Role of Attention , 1999, Perception.

[62]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  Yes It Can , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[63]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[64]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Awareness Becomes Necessary Between Adaptive Pattern Coding of Open and Closed Curvatures , 2011, Psychological science.

[66]  Ezequiel Morsella,et al.  The Unconscious Mind , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[67]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Primed numbers : Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming , 1999 .

[68]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[69]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  Glen E Bodner,et al.  Masked priming of number judgments depends on prime validity and task , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[71]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  W. Levelt On binocular rivalry , 1965 .

[73]  Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al.  Dissociable Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Conscious and Unconscious Control of Behavior , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[74]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[75]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Unconscious Automatic Processes: Evidence from Event-related Potentials in a Masked Priming Paradigm , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Parts Outweigh the Whole (Word) in Unconscious Analysis of Meaning , 2000, Psychological science.

[77]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[78]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Grabbing attention without knowing: Automatic capture of attention by subliminal spatial cues , 2007 .

[79]  R. Blake,et al.  Visually guided attention is neutralized when informative cues are visible but unperceived , 1993, Vision Research.

[80]  O. Neumann,et al.  Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking. , 1999 .

[81]  C. Paffen,et al.  Interocular conflict attracts attention , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[82]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single flash , 1984, Vision Research.

[83]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[84]  S. Tipper,et al.  Orienting attention via observed gaze shift evokes longer term inhibitory effects: implications for social interactions, attention, and memory. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[85]  E. Gordon,et al.  A Temporal Dissociation of Subliminal versus Supraliminal Fear Perception: An Event-related Potential Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[86]  Marvin M. Chun,et al.  Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning: Efficient Detection of Visual Regularities Without Awareness , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[88]  Susana Martinez-Conde,et al.  Dichoptic Visual Masking Reveals that Early Binocular Neurons Exhibit Weak Interocular Suppression: Implications for Binocular Vision and Visual Awareness , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[89]  C. Koch The quest for consciousness : a neurobiological approach , 2004 .

[90]  Peng Zhang,et al.  Voluntary Attention Modulates Processing of Eye-Specific Visual Information , 2012, Psychological science.

[91]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[92]  A. Kiesel,et al.  Follow the sign! Top-down contingent attentional capture of masked arrow cues , 2011, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[93]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Attention Speeds Binocular Rivalry , 2006, Psychological science.

[94]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[95]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[96]  C. Clifford Binocular rivalry , 2009, Current Biology.

[97]  Timothy J Andrews,et al.  Binocular rivalry and visual awareness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[98]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Learning to reach for ‘invisible’ visual input , 2011, Current Biology.

[99]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[100]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  Amygdala Responses to Fearful and Happy Facial Expressions under Conditions of Binocular Suppression , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[101]  N. Turk-Browne,et al.  Attention Is Spontaneously Biased Toward Regularities , 2013, Psychological science.