Seeing is believing: Utilization of subliminal symbols requires a visible relevant context
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] U. Mattler,et al. Masked stimuli modulate endogenous shifts of spatial attention , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.
[2] Brian N. Pasley,et al. Subcortical Discrimination of Unperceived Objects during Binocular Rivalry , 2004, Neuron.
[3] Fang Fang,et al. A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[4] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Event perception and the word repetition effect , 1988 .
[5] J. Hoffmann,et al. Mechanisms of subliminal response priming , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[6] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Suppressed images selectively affect the dominant percept during binocular rivalry. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[7] R. Blake,et al. Neural bases of binocular rivalry , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[8] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[9] M. Kiefer,et al. Attentional Modulation of Unconscious Automatic Processes: Evidence from Event-related Potentials in a Masked Priming Paradigm , 2006 .
[10] S J Luck,et al. Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] R. Shiffrin,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .
[12] Victor A. F. Lamme. Blindsight: the role of feedforward and feedback corticocortical connections. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[13] M. Posner,et al. Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .
[14] V. Lamme,et al. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[15] J. Pratt,et al. Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] S. Kouider,et al. Longer is not better: nonconscious overstimulation reverses priming influences under interocular suppression , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[17] R. Verleger,et al. How the Self Controls Its Automatic Pilot when Processing Subliminal Information , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[18] G. Horstmann,et al. Author ' s personal copy Attentional capture by masked colour singletons , 2010 .
[19] Sheng He,et al. Cortical Responses to Invisible Faces: Dissociating Subsystems for Facial-Information Processing , 2006, Current Biology.
[20] L. Zhaoping. Attention capture by eye of origin singletons even without awareness--a hallmark of a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[21] K. Forster,et al. Unconscious cognition isn’t that smart: Modulation of masked repetition priming effect in the word naming task , 2008, Cognition.
[22] J. Pratt,et al. Symbolic Control of Visual Attention , 2001, Psychological science.
[23] Lauri Nummenmaa,et al. Visuospatial attention shifts by gaze and arrow cues: An ERP study , 2008, Brain Research.
[24] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and performance IX , 1982 .
[25] P. McCormick. Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] P. Roelfsema,et al. A monocular, unconscious form of visual attention. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[27] R. Blake. A Neural Theory of Binocular Rivalry , 1989 .
[28] Sheng He,et al. Seeing the invisible: The scope and limits of unconscious processing in binocular rivalry , 2008 .
[29] N. Logothetis,et al. Local field potential reflects perceptual suppression in monkey visual cortex , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[30] Kenneth I Forster,et al. Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[31] R. Klein,et al. Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[32] M. Damian,et al. Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: automaticity rather than semantic processing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] M. Kiefer,et al. Time course of conscious and unconscious semantic brain activation , 2000, Neuroreport.
[34] Torsten Schubert,et al. Multiple bottlenecks in information processing? An electrophysiological examination , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[35] M. Aravindh. Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2010 .
[36] S. Dehaene,et al. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.
[37] Effective processing of masked eye gaze requires volitional control , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[38] Lauri Nummenmaa,et al. Gaze distractors influence saccadic curvature: Evidence for the role of the oculomotor system in gaze-cued orienting , 2006, Vision Research.
[39] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[40] Randolph Blake,et al. Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[41] F. Fang,et al. Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[42] Frank Tong,et al. Competing Theories of Binocular Rivalry: A Possible Resolution , 2001 .
[43] J. Hoffmann,et al. Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition , 2003, Cognition.
[44] O. Neumann,et al. Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cuing task. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] P. Calabresi,et al. Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] U. Ansorge,et al. Shifts of visuospatial attention to invisible (metacontrast-masked) singletons: Clues from reaction times and event-related potential , 2006 .
[47] B. Breitmeyer. Visual masking: past accomplishments, present status, future developments , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[48] Daniel Smilek,et al. Inhibition of return in response to gaze cues: The roles of time course and fixation cue , 2007 .
[49] M. Kiefer,et al. Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[50] Len Thomas,et al. Retrospective Power Analysis , 1997 .
[51] R. Blake. © 2001 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 5 A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies , 2000 .
[52] Alan Kingstone,et al. Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[53] J. Tipples. Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[54] A. Kingstone,et al. Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[55] Jonathan Winawer,et al. Gender aftereffects in the perception of silhouetted face profiles , 2010 .
[56] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Implicit Perceptual Anticipation Triggered by Statistical Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[57] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[58] P. Sterzer,et al. A direct oculomotor correlate of unconscious visual processing , 2012, Current Biology.
[59] J. Theeuwes,et al. The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[60] Martin Eimer,et al. Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[61] Zijiang J. He,et al. Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness: The Role of Attention , 1999, Perception.
[62] Ran R. Hassin,et al. Yes It Can , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[63] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[64] M. Eimer,et al. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[65] Marcia Grabowecky,et al. Awareness Becomes Necessary Between Adaptive Pattern Coding of Open and Closed Curvatures , 2011, Psychological science.
[66] Ezequiel Morsella,et al. The Unconscious Mind , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[67] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Primed numbers : Exploring the modularity of numerical representations with masked and unmasked semantic priming , 1999 .
[68] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[69] Alan Kingstone,et al. Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[70] Glen E Bodner,et al. Masked priming of number judgments depends on prime validity and task , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[71] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[72] W. Levelt. On binocular rivalry , 1965 .
[73] Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al. Dissociable Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Conscious and Unconscious Control of Behavior , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[74] J. Theeuwes,et al. Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[75] Markus Kiefer,et al. Attentional Modulation of Unconscious Automatic Processes: Evidence from Event-related Potentials in a Masked Priming Paradigm , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[76] Richard L. Abrams,et al. Parts Outweigh the Whole (Word) in Unconscious Analysis of Meaning , 2000, Psychological science.
[77] Jacob Cohen,et al. A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.
[78] J. Theeuwes,et al. Grabbing attention without knowing: Automatic capture of attention by subliminal spatial cues , 2007 .
[79] R. Blake,et al. Visually guided attention is neutralized when informative cues are visible but unperceived , 1993, Vision Research.
[80] O. Neumann,et al. Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking. , 1999 .
[81] C. Paffen,et al. Interocular conflict attracts attention , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[82] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single flash , 1984, Vision Research.
[83] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[84] S. Tipper,et al. Orienting attention via observed gaze shift evokes longer term inhibitory effects: implications for social interactions, attention, and memory. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[85] E. Gordon,et al. A Temporal Dissociation of Subliminal versus Supraliminal Fear Perception: An Event-related Potential Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[86] Marvin M. Chun,et al. Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning: Efficient Detection of Visual Regularities Without Awareness , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[87] S. Yantis,et al. Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.
[88] Susana Martinez-Conde,et al. Dichoptic Visual Masking Reveals that Early Binocular Neurons Exhibit Weak Interocular Suppression: Implications for Binocular Vision and Visual Awareness , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[89] C. Koch. The quest for consciousness : a neurobiological approach , 2004 .
[90] Peng Zhang,et al. Voluntary Attention Modulates Processing of Eye-Specific Visual Information , 2012, Psychological science.
[91] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[92] A. Kiesel,et al. Follow the sign! Top-down contingent attentional capture of masked arrow cues , 2011, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[93] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Attention Speeds Binocular Rivalry , 2006, Psychological science.
[94] S. Dehaene,et al. Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[95] J. Jonides. Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .
[96] C. Clifford. Binocular rivalry , 2009, Current Biology.
[97] Timothy J Andrews,et al. Binocular rivalry and visual awareness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[98] Derek H. Arnold,et al. Learning to reach for ‘invisible’ visual input , 2011, Current Biology.
[99] Martin Eimer,et al. Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[100] Adam P. Morris,et al. Amygdala Responses to Fearful and Happy Facial Expressions under Conditions of Binocular Suppression , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[101] N. Turk-Browne,et al. Attention Is Spontaneously Biased Toward Regularities , 2013, Psychological science.