Examining the robustness of observational associations to model, measurement and sampling uncertainty with the vibration of effects framework

BACKGROUND The results of studies on observational associations may vary depending on the study design and analysis choices as well as due to measurement error. It is important to understand the relative contribution of different factors towards generating variable results, including low sample sizes, researchers' flexibility in model choices, and measurement error in variables of interest and adjustment variables. METHODS We define sampling, model and measurement uncertainty, and extend the concept of vibration of effects in order to study these three types of uncertainty in a common framework. In a practical application, we examine these types of uncertainty in a Cox model using data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. In addition, we analyse the behaviour of sampling, model and measurement uncertainty for varying sample sizes in a simulation study. RESULTS All types of uncertainty are associated with a potentially large variability in effect estimates. Measurement error in the variable of interest attenuates the true effect in most cases, but can occasionally lead to overestimation. When we consider measurement error in both the variable of interest and adjustment variables, the vibration of effects are even less predictable as both systematic under- and over-estimation of the true effect can be observed. The results on simulated data show that measurement and model vibration remain non-negligible even for large sample sizes. CONCLUSION Sampling, model and measurement uncertainty can have important consequences for the stability of observational associations. We recommend systematically studying and reporting these types of uncertainty, and comparing them in a common framework.

[1]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  The False-positive to False-negative Ratio in Epidemiologic Studies , 2011, Epidemiology.

[2]  John P A Ioannidis,et al.  Is everything we eat associated with cancer? A systematic cookbook review. , 2013, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[3]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  Registration practices for observational studies on ClinicalTrials.gov indicated low adherence. , 2016, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[4]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  Vibration of effects in epidemiologic studies of alcohol consumption and breast cancer risk. , 2020, International journal of epidemiology.

[5]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  Consideration of confounding was suboptimal in the reporting of observational studies in psychiatry: a meta-epidemiological study. , 2019, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[6]  Anne-Laure Boulesteix,et al.  Comparing the vibration of effects due to model, data pre-processing and sampling uncertainty on a large data set in personality psychology , 2020, Meta-Psychology.

[7]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  Interpretation of epidemiologic studies very often lacked adequate consideration of confounding. , 2018, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[8]  Maarten van Smeden,et al.  Measurement error is often neglected in medical literature: a systematic review. , 2018, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[9]  Adrian E. Raftery,et al.  Bayesian Model Averaging: A Tutorial , 2016 .

[10]  Paolo Boffetta,et al.  False-Positive Results in Cancer Epidemiology: A Plea for Epistemological Modesty , 2008, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[11]  Sabine Hoffmann,et al.  Shared and unshared exposure measurement error in occupational cohort studies and their effects on statistical inference in proportional hazards models , 2018, PloS one.

[12]  John P A Ioannidis,et al.  Assessment of vibration of effects due to model specification can demonstrate the instability of observational associations. , 2015, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[13]  M. Wong,et al.  Correlated measurement error--implications for nutritional epidemiology. , 2004, International journal of epidemiology.

[14]  B G Armstrong,et al.  Effect of measurement error on epidemiological studies of environmental and occupational exposures. , 1998, Occupational and environmental medicine.

[15]  D. Thomas,et al.  Exposure measurement error: influence on exposure-disease. Relationships and methods of correction. , 1993, Annual review of public health.

[16]  S Richardson,et al.  A Bayesian approach to measurement error problems in epidemiology using conditional independence models. , 1993, American journal of epidemiology.

[17]  Adrian E. Raftery,et al.  Bayesian model averaging: a tutorial (with comments by M. Clyde, David Draper and E. I. George, and a rejoinder by the authors , 1999 .

[18]  J. Ioannidis Why Most Discovered True Associations Are Inflated , 2008, Epidemiology.

[19]  Timothy L Lash,et al.  The Harm Done to Reproducibility by the Culture of Null Hypothesis Significance Testing. , 2017, American journal of epidemiology.

[20]  Cosetta Minelli,et al.  Systematic review of statistical approaches to quantify, or correct for, measurement error in a continuous exposure in nutritional epidemiology , 2017, BMC Medical Research Methodology.

[21]  J. Ioannidis,et al.  Field-wide meta-analyses of observational associations can map selective availability of risk factors and the impact of model specifications. , 2016, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[22]  Andrew Gelman,et al.  Measurement error and the replication crisis , 2017, Science.

[23]  Maarten van Smeden,et al.  Random measurement error: Why worry? An example of cardiovascular risk factors , 2018, PloS one.

[24]  V. Kipnis,et al.  Impact of exposure measurement error in nutritional epidemiology. , 2008, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[25]  N. Day,et al.  The effect of correlated measurement error in multivariate models of diet. , 2004, American journal of epidemiology.

[26]  G. Taubes Epidemiology faces its limits. , 1995, Science.

[27]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Multimodel Inference , 2004 .