Mapping Attitude Formation as a Function of Information Input: Online Processing Models of Attitude Formation

Consistent with calls for a dynamical social psychology (Jacoby et al., 1987; Nowak, Lewenstein, & Vallacher, 1994), attitude formation was studied using a recently developed, computer-based simulation technique termed Higher Order Cognitive Tracing (see Jacoby et al., 1994). Participants’ attitudes toward 12 different products in 3 product categories were investigated as a function of incremental information input. As opposed to traditional memory-based models of attitude formation, the study explored online processing models. Results indicate that the impact of information tends to decrease the later in the sequence that information is accessed. In addition, new information that is affectively inconsistent with prior information tends to have a greater impact on attitudes than information that is affectively redundant. This effect is more pronounced earlier rather than later in the sequence of information acquisition. Implications and suggestions for future research are discussed.

[1]  James Jaccard,et al.  Attitudes and behavior: An information integration perspective , 1985 .

[2]  William A. Fisher,et al.  Pre-Purchase Information Acquisition: Description of a Process Methodology, Research Paradigm, and Pilot Investigation , 1976 .

[3]  Gregory R. Wood,et al.  An idiothetic analysis of consumer decision making , 1986 .

[4]  Stephen J. Misovich,et al.  Methodological Approaches to the Study of Social Event Perception , 1994 .

[5]  Methods for identifying consequential beliefs: Implications for understanding attitude strength , 1995 .

[6]  R. Petty,et al.  Attitude strength : antecedents and consequences , 1995 .

[7]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Part-Whole Question Sequences: A Conversational Logic Analysis , 1991 .

[8]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  K. Gergen Social Psychology as History , 1973 .

[10]  M. Fishbein,et al.  Inferential beliefs and order effects in personality impression formation , 1975 .

[11]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Cognitive theories of persuasion , 1984 .

[12]  J. Jacoby Consumer psychology as a social psychological sphere of action. , 1975 .

[13]  Gita Venkataramani Johar,et al.  A Varying-Parameter Averaging Model of On-Line Brand Evaluations , 1997 .

[14]  David Mazursky,et al.  New directions in behavioral process research: Implications for social psychology , 1987 .

[15]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[16]  N. Anderson Integration theory and attitude change. , 1971 .

[17]  T. Ostrom,et al.  Amount of information and interpersonal judgment. , 1974 .

[18]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Intrinsic Dynamics of Social Judgment , 1994 .

[19]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[20]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  The chaos in social psychology. , 1994 .

[21]  Jacob Jacoby,et al.  Tracing the Impact of Item-by-Item Information Accessing on Uncertainty Reduction , 1994 .

[22]  T. Kuhn The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. , 1964 .

[23]  David Mazursky,et al.  Effectiveness of security analyst information accessing strategies: A computer interactive assessment , 1985 .

[24]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Dynamical Systems in Social Psychology , 1994 .

[25]  Ma. de la Natividad Jiménez Salas,et al.  The Conduct of Inquiry , 1967 .

[26]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches , 1981 .

[27]  N. Anderson Foundations of information integration theory , 1981 .

[28]  K. Deaux Reconstructing Social Identity , 1993 .

[29]  S. Himmelfarb General test of a differential weighted averaging model of impression formation , 1973 .

[30]  W. Mcguire Some Internal Psychological Factors Influencing Consumer Choice , 1976 .

[31]  James Jaccard,et al.  Idiothetic methods for the analysis of behavioral decision making: Computer applications , 1988 .

[32]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1993 .

[33]  W. Mcguire The Yin and Yang of Progress in Social Psychology: Seven Koan. , 1973 .

[34]  N. Anderson,et al.  Belief revision in children: serial judgment in social cognition and decision-making domains. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Wayne D. Hoyer,et al.  Psychometric Characteristics of Behavioral Process Data: Preliminary Findings on Validity and Reliability , 1978 .

[36]  R. Hastie,et al.  The relationship between memory and judgment depends on whether the judgment task is memory-based or on-line , 1986 .

[37]  Ralph L. Rosnow Paradigms in Transition: The Methodology of Social Inquiry , 1981 .

[38]  David Brinberg,et al.  Perspectives on methodology in consumer research , 1986 .

[39]  Joseph L. Zinnes,et al.  Theory and Methods of Scaling. , 1958 .

[40]  Gardner Murphy,et al.  Experimental social psychology. , 1934 .

[41]  Norman H. Anderson,et al.  Contributions to information integration theory , 1991 .

[42]  M. Zanna,et al.  Attitudes and Attitude Change , 1993 .

[43]  Jacob Jacoby,et al.  The Emerging Behavioral Process Technology in Consumer Decision-Making Research , 1977 .

[44]  J. Jacoby,et al.  Time and Consumer Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Overview , 1976 .

[45]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Uneven progress: Social psychology and the study of attitudes. , 1992 .