Landmark discrimination learning in the dog: effects of age, an antioxidant fortified food, and cognitive strategy

The landmark discrimination learning test can be used to assess the ability to utilize allocentric spatial information to locate targets. The present experiments examined the role of various factors on performance of a landmark discrimination learning task in beagle dogs. Experiments 1 and 2 looked at the effects of age and food composition. Experiments 3 and 4 were aimed at characterizing the cognitive strategies used in performance on this task and in long-term retention. Cognitively equivalent groups of old and young dogs were placed into either a test group maintained on food enriched with a broad-spectrum of antioxidants and mitochondrial cofactors, or a control group maintained on a complete and balanced food formulated for adult dogs. Following a wash-in period, the dogs were tested on a series of problems, in which reward was obtained when the animal responded selectively to the object closest to a thin wooden block, which served as a landmark. In Experiment 1, dogs were first trained to respond to a landmark placed directly on top of coaster, landmark 0 (L0). In the next phase of testing, the landmark was moved at successively greater distances (1, 4 or 10 cm) away from the reward object. Learning varied as a function of age group, food group, and task. The young dogs learned all of the tasks more quickly than the old dogs. The aged dogs on the enriched food learned L0 significantly more rapidly than aged dogs on control food. A higher proportion of dogs on the enriched food learned the task, when the distance was increased to 1cm. Experiment 2 showed that accuracy decreased with increased distance between the reward object and landmark, and this effect was greater in old animals. Experiment 3 showed stability of performance, despite using a novel landmark, and new locations, indicating that dogs learned the landmark concept. Experiment 4 found age impaired long-term retention of the landmark task. These results indicate that allocentric spatial learning is impaired in an age-dependent manner in dogs, and that age also affects performance when the distance between the landmark and target is increased. In addition, these results both support a role of oxidative damage in the development of age-associated cognitive dysfunction and indicate that short-term administration of a food enriched with supplemental antioxidants and mitochondrial cofactors can partially reverse the deleterious effects of aging on cognition.

[1]  D. Amaral,et al.  Evidence for task-dependent memory dysfunction in the aged monkey , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  C. Cotman,et al.  Dietary enrichment counteracts age-associated cognitive dysfunction in canines , 2002, Neurobiology of Aging.

[3]  B. Ames,et al.  Oxidative damage and mitochondrial decay in aging. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  B. Ames,et al.  Oxidants Are a Major Contributor to Aging a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[5]  N. Inestrosa,et al.  The role of oxidative stress in the toxicity induced by amyloid β-peptide in Alzheimer’s disease , 2000, Progress in Neurobiology.

[6]  J. A. Roberts,et al.  Impaired spatial information processing in aged monkeys with preserved recognition memory , 1997, Neuroreport.

[7]  P. Riederer,et al.  Alpha-lipoic acid as a new treatment option for Alzheimer [corrected] type dementia. , 2001, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[8]  R. Killiany,et al.  Executive system dysfunction in the aged monkey: Spatial and object reversal learning , 1995, Neurobiology of Aging.

[9]  B. Ames,et al.  Memory loss in old rats is associated with brain mitochondrial decay and RNA/DNA oxidation: Partial reversal by feeding acetyl-l-carnitine and/or R-α-lipoic acid , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  C. Cotman,et al.  Visuospatial impairments in aged canines (Canis familiaris): the role of cognitive-behavioral flexibility. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  R. Floyd,et al.  Reversal of age-related increase in brain protein oxidation, decrease in enzyme activity, and loss in temporal and spatial memory by chronic administration of the spin-trapping compound N-tert-butyl-alpha-phenylnitrone. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  M. Beal,et al.  Aging, energy, and oxidative stress in neurodegenerative diseases , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[13]  C. Cotman,et al.  Use of a delayed non-matching to position task to model age-dependent cognitive decline in the dog , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  R. Biegler,et al.  Landmark stability is a prerequisite for spatial but not discrimination learning , 1993, Nature.

[15]  M. Moss The longitudinal assessment of recognition memory in aged rhesus monkeys , 1993, Neurobiology of Aging.

[16]  Brian J Cummings,et al.  β-Amyloid Accumulation Correlates with Cognitive Dysfunction in the Aged Canine , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[17]  R. Killiany,et al.  Patterns of cognitive decline in aged rhesus monkeys , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Aged monkeys exhibit behavioral deficits indicative of widespread cerebral dysfunction , 1991, Neurobiology of Aging.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Decreases in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow with Normal Aging , 1991, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[20]  J. Bachevalier Behavioral changes in aged rhesus monkeys , 1993, Neurobiology of Aging.

[21]  M. Voytko Impairments in acquisition and reversals of two-choice discriminations by aged rhesus monkeys , 1999, Neurobiology of Aging.

[22]  Douglas L. Rosene,et al.  Effects of aging on visual recognition memory in the rhesus monkey , 1988, Neurobiology of Aging.

[23]  J. Hodges,et al.  The fractionation of remote memory. Evidence from a longitudinal study of dementia of Alzheimer type. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  A. Katz,et al.  Remote memory: recalling autobiographical and public events from across the lifespan. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[25]  L. Balázs,et al.  Evidence of an oxidative challenge in the Alzheimer's brain , 1994, Neurochemical Research.

[26]  B. Ames Micronutrients prevent cancer and delay aging. , 1998, Toxicology letters.

[27]  B. Ames,et al.  Oxidants, antioxidants, and the degenerative diseases of aging. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Brian J Cummings,et al.  Spatial learning and memory as a function of age in the dog. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  K. Rosenblum,et al.  Lateral ventricle injection of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin impairs long-term memory in a spatial memory task , 1998, Brain Research.

[30]  P. Bickford,et al.  Long-Term Dietary Strawberry, Spinach, or Vitamin E Supplementation Retards the Onset of Age-Related Neuronal Signal-Transduction and Cognitive Behavioral Deficits , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  C. Napoli,et al.  Chronic antioxidant supplementation attenuates nuclear factor-kappa B activation and preserves endothelial function in hypercholesterolemic pigs. , 2002, Cardiovascular research.

[32]  B. Ames,et al.  Acetyl-L-carnitine fed to old rats partially restores mitochondrial function and ambulatory activity. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  B. Shukitt-Hale,et al.  Neurobehavioral aspects of antioxidants in aging , 2000, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[34]  B. Shukitt-Hale,et al.  Oxidative stress protection and vulnerability in aging: putative nutritional implications for intervention , 2000, Mechanisms of Ageing and Development.

[35]  B. Ames,et al.  The free radical theory of aging matures. , 1998, Physiological reviews.

[36]  Landmark discrimination in the rat: a measure of allocentric spatial ability. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[37]  C. Cotman,et al.  Visual-discrimination learning ability and β-amyloid accumulation in the dog , 1998, Neurobiology of Aging.

[38]  L. Bäckman Memory functioning in dementia , 1992 .

[39]  Patrick R. Hof,et al.  Functional neurobiology of aging , 2001 .

[40]  T. Kikusui,et al.  Age-related working memory deficits in the allocentric place discrimination task: possible involvement in cholinergic dysfunction , 1999, Neurobiology of Aging.

[41]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Dissociation of the medial prefrontal, posterior parietal, and posterior temporal cortex for spatial navigation and recognition memory in the rat. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  G. Arendash,et al.  Chronic antioxidant treatment improves the cognitive performance of aged rats , 1995, Brain Research.

[43]  M. Lynch,et al.  Age-related changes in LTP and antioxidant defenses are reversed by an alpha-lipoic acid-enriched diet. , 1999, Neurobiology of aging.

[44]  D. Hughes,et al.  Alpha-lipoic acid modulates NF-κB activity in human monocytic cells by direct interaction with DNA , 2002, Experimental Gerontology.

[45]  E. Stadtman,et al.  Protein Oxidation in Aging, Disease, and Oxidative Stress* , 1997, The Journal of Biological Chemistry.

[46]  R. Bartus,et al.  Aging in the rhesus monkey: effects on visual discrimination learning and reversal learning. , 1979, Journal of gerontology.

[47]  W. Pohl,et al.  Dissociation of spatial discrimination deficits following frontal and parietal lesions in monkeys. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[48]  Norton W. Milgram,et al.  Development of a protocol for studying object recognition memory in the dog , 2000, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[49]  N. Milgram,et al.  Cognitive functions and aging in the dog: acquisition of nonspatial visual tasks , 1994 .

[50]  P. Bickford,et al.  Antioxidant-rich diets improve cerebellar physiology and motor learning in aged rats , 2000, Brain Research.

[51]  W. Markesbery,et al.  Increased Nuclear DNA Oxidation in the Brain in Alzheimer's Disease , 1998, Journal of neurochemistry.

[52]  Visual discrimination and reversal learning in the aged monkey (Macaca mulatta). , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.

[53]  A Bast,et al.  The pharmacology of the antioxidant lipoic acid. , 1997, General pharmacology.

[54]  A. Cronin-Golomb Color Vision, Object Recognition, and Spatial Localization in Aging and Alzheimer's Disease , 2001 .

[55]  B. Ames,et al.  (R)‐α‐Lipoic acid‐supplemented old rats have improved mitochondrial function, decreased oxidative damage, and increased metabolic rate , 1999, FASEB journal : official publication of the Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology.

[56]  H R Johnson,et al.  Aging in the rhesus monkey: debilitating effects on short-term memory. , 1978, Journal of gerontology.

[57]  S. Kojima,et al.  Object discrimination learning in aged Japanese monkeys. , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[58]  J. Termini,et al.  Oxidative base damage in RNA detected by reverse transcriptase. , 1995, Nucleic acids research.

[59]  J. Fallon,et al.  In search of molecular memory: experience-driven protein synthesis , 2000, Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences CMLS.

[60]  J. Aggleton,et al.  A comparison of egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in a patient with selective hippocampal damage , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  D. Harman Aging: a theory based on free radical and radiation chemistry. , 1956, Journal of gerontology.

[62]  C Beaulieu,et al.  Spatial encoding of hidden objects in dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[63]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Effects of object preferences and aversions on discrimination learning in monkeys with frontal lesions. , 1961 .

[64]  Seth J. Ramus,et al.  Severity of memory impairment in monkeys as a function of locus and extent of damage within the medial temporal lobe memory system , 1994, Hippocampus.

[65]  N. Milgram,et al.  The canine as a model of human cognitive aging: Recent developments , 2000, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[66]  David W. Killilea,et al.  Age-associated mitochondrial oxidative decay: Improvement of carnitine acetyltransferase substrate-binding affinity and activity in brain by feeding old rats acetyl-l- carnitine and/or R-α-lipoic acid , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[67]  A. Wu,et al.  Automated assay of oxygen radical absorbance capacity with the COBAS FARA II. , 1995, Clinical chemistry.

[68]  W. Markesbery,et al.  Four-Hydroxynonenal, a Product of Lipid Peroxidation, is Increased in the Brain in Alzheimer’s Disease , 1998, Neurobiology of Aging.

[69]  C. Cotman,et al.  Landmark discrimination learning in the dog. , 1999, Learning & memory.

[70]  A Begega,et al.  Effects of ageing on allocentric and egocentric spatial strategies in the Wistar rat , 2001, Behavioural Processes.