Instructional Complexity and the Science to Constrain It

School-researcher partnerships and large in vivo experiments help focus on useful, effective, instruction. Science and technology have had enormous impact on many areas of human endeavor but surprisingly little effect on education. Many large-scale field trials of science-based innovations in education have yielded scant evidence of improvement in student learning (1, 2), although a few have reliable positive outcomes (3, 4). Education involves many important issues, such as cultural questions of values, but we focus on instructional decision-making in the context of determined instructional goals and suggests ways to manage instructional complexity.

[1]  A. Paivio Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning , 1965 .

[2]  Kurt VanLehn,et al.  Learning one Subprocedure per Lesson , 1987, Artif. Intell..

[3]  R. Giere Cognitive Models of Science , 1992 .

[4]  R. Schmidt,et al.  New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .

[5]  E. Gorzelańczyk,et al.  Optimization of repetition spacing in the practice of learning. , 1994, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[6]  Phillip L. Ackerman,et al.  An aptitude–treatment interaction approach to transfer within training. , 1996 .

[7]  Andrew Sears,et al.  Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , 2002, CHI 2002.

[8]  C. Shea,et al.  Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Dirk van Rijn,et al.  Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 2003 .

[10]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  The Expertise Reversal Effect , 2003 .

[11]  David Klahr,et al.  Cognitive Research and Elementary Science Instruction: From the Laboratory, to the Classroom, and Back , 2005 .

[12]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The Transfer of Scientific Principles Using Concrete and Idealized Simulations , 2005, Journal of the Learning Sciences.

[13]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Enhancing Instructional Efficiency of Interactive E-learning Environments: A Cognitive Load Perspective , 2007 .

[14]  William R. Penuel,et al.  Effectiveness of Reading and Mathematics Software Products: Findings from the First Student Cohort , 2007 .

[15]  Vladimir M Sloutsky,et al.  The Advantage of Abstract Examples in Learning Math , 2008, Science.

[16]  John R Anderson,et al.  Using a model to compute the optimal schedule of practice. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[17]  Jeremy Roschelle,et al.  Integration of Technology, Curriculum, and Professional Development for Advancing Middle School Mathematics , 2010 .

[18]  Albert T. Corbett,et al.  The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction Framework: Bridging the Science-Practice Chasm to Enhance Robust Student Learning , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[19]  Kenneth R. Koedinger,et al.  Problem Order Implications for Learning Transfer , 2012, ITS.

[20]  R. Bjork,et al.  Self-regulated learning: beliefs, techniques, and illusions. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[21]  D. Klahr What do we mean? On the importance of not abandoning scientific rigor when talking about science education , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  John Dunlosky,et al.  Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising Directions From Cognitive and Educational Psychology , 2012 .

[23]  Daniel F. McCaffrey,et al.  Effectiveness of Cognitive Tutor Algebra I at Scale , 2014 .

[24]  Christa S. C. Asterhan,et al.  Socializing Intelligence Through Academic Talk and Dialogue , 2015 .