Does eye gaze indicate implicit knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge.

Three-year-olds sometimes look to the correct location but give an incorrect verbal answer in a false belief task. We examined whether correct eye gaze among 3- to 5-year-old children indexed unconscious knowledge or low confidence conscious knowledge. Children "bet" counters on where they thought a story character would go. If children were conscious of the knowledge conveyed by their eye gaze then they should have bet modestly on their explicit answer (i.e., been unsure whether this answer or the answer conveyed through eye direction was correct). We found that children bet very highly on the location consistent with their explicit answer, suggesting that they were not aware of the knowledge conveyed through their eye gaze. This result was supported by a number of conditions that showed that betting was a sensitive measure of even small degrees of uncertainty. The results shed light on false-belief understanding, the implicit-explicit distinction, and transitional knowledge. We argue that the transition to a full understanding of false belief is marked by periods of implicit knowledge and explicit understanding with low confidence.

[1]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[2]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[3]  Cognitive Certainty in Children: Effects of Concept, Developmental Level, and Method of Assessment. , 1977 .

[4]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[5]  P. Merikle,et al.  Priming with and without awareness , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  L. Acredolo,et al.  The role of attentiveness, mobility history, and separation of hiding sites on stage IV search behavior , 1986 .

[7]  D. Rosenthal Two concepts of consciousness , 1986 .

[8]  O. Huber,et al.  Development of the concept of comparative subjective probability , 1987 .

[9]  R. Baillargeon Object permanence in 3½- and 4½-month-old infants. , 1987 .

[10]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[11]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[12]  A. Diamond,et al.  Abilities and neural mechanisms underlying AB performance. , 1988, Child development.

[13]  Renee L Baillargeon,et al.  Location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task: Further evidence , 1989 .

[14]  Eric A. Jenkins,et al.  How Children Discover New Strategies , 1989 .

[15]  A. Diamond Developmental Time Course in Human Infants and Infant Monkeys, and the Neural Bases of, Inhibitory Control in Reaching a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Implicit Learning , 1993 .

[17]  M. Alibali,et al.  Gesture-Speech Mismatch and Mechanisms of Learning: What the Hands Reveal about a Child′s State of Mind , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  The cognizer's innards: A psychological and philosophical perspective on the development of thought. , 1993 .

[19]  D. Dennett Learning and Labeling , 1993 .

[20]  D. Shanks,et al.  Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  L. Schauble,et al.  Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. , 1994 .

[22]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Dissociable definitions of consciousness , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[23]  Josef Perner,et al.  Implicit Understanding of Belief , 1994 .

[24]  John C. Burnham Minds in the making , 1994, Nature.

[25]  Z. Dienes,et al.  UNCONSCIOUS KNOWLEDGE OF ARTIFICIAL GRAMMARS IS APPLIED STRATEGICALLY , 1995 .

[26]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[27]  T. Ruffman Do Children Understand the Mind by Means of Simulation or a Theory? Evidence From Their Understanding of Inference , 1996 .

[28]  P. Zelazo,et al.  An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them ☆ , 1996 .

[29]  J. Reznick,et al.  Response modality affects human infant delayed-response performance. , 1996, Child development.

[30]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  Language, Thought and Consciousness: An Essay in Philosophical Psychology , 1996 .

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Rethinking infant knowledge: toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. , 1997, Psychological review.

[32]  Hosenfeld,et al.  Indicators of Discontinuous Change in the Development of Analogical Reasoning , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[33]  T. Ruffman,et al.  Why do infants make A not B errors in a search task, yet show memory for the location of hidden objects in a nonsearch task? , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[34]  Z. Dienes,et al.  A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[35]  Verbal imprecision as an index of knowledge in transition. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[36]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Beyond words: the importance of gesture to researchers and learners. , 2000, Child development.

[37]  W. Clements,et al.  Promoting the Transition from Implicit to Explicit Understanding: A Training Study of False Belief , 2000 .

[38]  T. Ruffman,et al.  Doesn’t see, doesn’t know: is anticipatory looking really related to understanding or belief? , 2001 .