Contexts and catalysts

There has been a historical tension between theories of brain function emphasizing regional specialization and those focusing on integration across regions. This tension continues despite the pervasive use of functional neuroimaging, which enables testing of these theories in the human brain. There are instances of agreement, where regions thought to be critical for a given behavior (e.g., Broca’s area and language production) do become more active when a person engages in that behavior. However, a number of disconcerting results have also been found. These include activation in areas not thought to be important for the behavior, and lack of activation in regions thought to be critical for particular behaviors based on studies of the damaged brain. A recently proposed Neural Context hypothesis of brain function provides a mechanism that can reconcile these apparently disparate findings. The hypothesis states that the functional relevance of a brain area depends on the status of other connected areas—i.e., the context within which the region is operating. A region can participate in several behaviors through variations in its interactions with other areas. It is possible that certain critical nodes serve as Behavioural Catalysts, enabling the transition between behavioral states, without differential alterations in the measured activity. By virtue of its anatomical connections, an area could facilitate a shift in functional connectivity between one set of regions to another. An imaging study on the changing interregional interactions involving the hippocampus in learning and awareness serves as an example of neural context. In this case, the hippocampus may serve to catalyze the transition to awareness.

[1]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  Karl J. Friston Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: A synthesis , 1994 .

[3]  L. Squire Memory and Brain , 1987 .

[4]  G Tononi,et al.  Measures of degeneracy and redundancy in biological networks. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  A R McIntosh,et al.  Functional interactions between the medial temporal lobes and posterior neocortex related to episodic memory retrieval. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Degeneracy and redundancy in cognitive anatomy , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Preface: Cerebral Cortex Has Come of Age , 1991 .

[9]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[11]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[12]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Neural modeling, functional brain imaging, and cognition , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  G. Ermentrout Principles of brain functioning: A synergetic approach to brain activity, behavior, and cognition , 1997 .

[14]  G Tononi,et al.  A complexity measure for selective matching of signals by the brain. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  Michael C. Mackey,et al.  Neural ensemble coding and statistical periodicity: Speculations on the operation of the mind's eye , 2000, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[16]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Functional Connectivity of the Medial Temporal Lobe Relates to Learning and Awareness , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  F. Wörgötter,et al.  Context, state and the receptive fields of striatal cortex cells , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  J. Donoghue,et al.  Dynamic organization of primary motor cortex output to target muscles in adult rats II. Rapid reorganization following motor nerve lesions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  G. Edelman,et al.  A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  M. Young,et al.  Sparse population coding of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. , 1992, Science.

[21]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Network analysis of cortical visual pathways mapped with PET , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Lateralized Cognitive Processes and Lateralized Task Control in the Human Brain , 2003, Science.

[23]  Jonathan R. Wolpaw,et al.  The complex structure of a simple memory , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic representations and generative models of brain function , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[25]  F L Bookstein,et al.  Differential functional connectivity of prefrontal and medial temporal cortices during episodic memory retrieval , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[26]  D. Poeppel A Critical Review of PET Studies of Phonological Processing , 1996, Brain and Language.

[27]  Robert E. Clark,et al.  Awareness and the Conditioned Eyeblink Response , 2002 .

[28]  G. Edelman Group selection and phasic reentrant signaling a theory of higher brain function , 1982 .

[29]  J. Kelso,et al.  Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  M. A. O'Neil,et al.  The connectional organization of the cortico-thalamic system of the cat. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Neurobiology of conscious experience , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Changes in limbic and prefrontal functional interactions in a working memory task for faces. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  M. Young,et al.  Computational analysis of functional connectivity between areas of primate cerebral cortex. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[34]  Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al.  Towards a network theory of cognition , 2000, Neural Networks.

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Variability in fMRI: An Examination of Intersession Differences , 2000, NeuroImage.

[36]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. , 1999, Memory.

[37]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Connectivity and complexity: the relationship between neuroanatomy and brain dynamics , 2000, Neural Networks.

[38]  J. Wolpaw,et al.  Memory traces in primate spinal cord produced by operant conditioning of H-reflex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  R. Clark,et al.  Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. , 1998, Science.

[40]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Human Prefrontal Cortex Is Not Specific for Working Memory: A Functional MRI Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[41]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  A. P. Kozlov,et al.  Analysis of the Dynamics of Interneuronal Functional Connections during Conditioned Reflex Activity , 2000, Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology.